Skip to main content

Effects of fruit and vegetable intake on memory and attention: a systematic review of randomized controlled trials

Abstract

Background

Memory and attention are important for daily functioning, and their function deteriorates due to aging. However, fruit and vegetable consumption are one of the protective factors against deterioration in memory and attention. This systematic review of randomized controlled trials (RCTs) aims to identify the effects of fruit and vegetable consumption on memory and attention.

Methods

We conducted a systematic search in EBSCOhost, ProQuest, PubMed, Embase, and Web of Science from inception up to 06/09/2022. The inclusion criteria were peer-reviewed articles, fruit and vegetable intake measured using randomized controlled trials, and the outcome measures that showed the results of memory and attention scores. Two researchers independently extracted articles that met the selection criteria and evaluated the quality of each study.

Results

There were 70 articles identified from the databases, of which 13 articles met the inclusion criteria and were included in this systematic review. There were 493 participants in total. The results show that consumption of fruit and vegetable intake improved memory and attention in longitudinal studies (10 to 12 weeks). Children showed improvement in immediate recall after supplementation with blueberries. Older adults required a higher dose of fruit and vegetable intake consumption to achieve significant improvement compared with children and younger adults. Furthermore, the effect of fruits and vegetables on memory showed better immediate memory recall than delayed recall.

Conclusion

This systematic review showed that there is an improvement in memory and attention with fruit and vegetable intake consumption. Hence, awareness of fruit and vegetable intake consumption is important to maintain cognitive health.

Peer Review reports

Introduction

Fruits and vegetables have numerous nutritional benefits, such as high concentrations of nutrients, including vitamins, minerals, fibre, carotenoids, flavonoids, and phytochemicals [1,2,3]. Phytochemicals play a nutraceutical role [4] and function as antioxidants and scavengers for free radicals that have been shown to slow cognitive deterioration [5]. Fruits such as blueberries, cherries, mulberries, grapes, and Lycium barbarum (goji berries) contain dietary polyphenols [6,7,8,9] and phytochemicals [10], which are associated with delay of the onset of cognitive decline [11].

Cognitive decline heralds dementia, which is a global issue now where the World Health Organization (WHO) has projected that 152 million people will have dementia in 2050 [12]. Dementia symptoms include gradual, progressive memory loss and attention deficits [13]. Individuals who lose attention have difficulty learning new things and trouble reading long texts. Furthermore, individuals (studies including patients only or including nonpatients as well) with memory loss, such as episodic memory loss, tended to forget daily events includes taking medicine or appointments. In summary, cognitive decline affects an individual’s daily functioning [14, 15] and workplace performance [16]. One of the reasons for memory deterioration is brain aging, especially oxidative stress in the hippocampus [17, 18], as the hippocampus stores long-term memory [19]. In addition to memory loss, dementia patients also suffer from attention deficits. Attention involves various anatomical areas, including the thalamus and the occipital, temporal, parietal, and frontal cortices, which form a neural network [20]. The attention neural network is influenced by the rates of neuron firing [21, 22], and unfortunately, demyelination occurs due to aging, which slows information processing [23, 24] and affects attention.

The theoretical framework guiding this study is the Scaffolding Theory of Aging and Cognition (STAC) [25, 26]. STAC is a conceptual model that integrates biological aging and environmental factors that affect cognitive function and the dynamic interaction with protective factors and compensatory process in the brain. The theory suggests that as we age, our brains develop compensatory mechanisms, which are referred to as scaffolds, to maintain cognitive function, countering the effects physiological aging [25, 27] (refer to Fig. 1). One of the compensatory mechanisms is lifestyle choices, such as dietary intake. Previous studies have shown that an adequate amount of fruit and vegetable intake is an effective preventive measure against cognitive decline [28,29,30]. Fruits and vegetables are rich in neuroprotective compounds and may act as cognitive scaffolds in the context of STAC.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Conceptual model of the Scaffolding Theory of Aging and Cognition (STAC) [25]

Various forms of fruit and vegetable intake have been explored in previous studies, for example, in the form of capsules [31, 32], juices [33,34,35,36,37,38], powder [39,40,41], and frozen fruits [42]. Additionally, the efficacy of cognitive function was also measured by different cognitive tests. For example, memory is measured by the California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT) [35], the Computerized Mental Performance Assessment System (COMPASS) [33], the Visual Verbal Learning Test (VVLT) [36], the Spatial Paired Associate Learning Test [35], and the Visual Spatial Learning Test [36]. Furthermore, attention is also measured by different methods, e.g. the Stroop test [31, 37, 40, 43], Frankfurt Attention Inventory [32], Trail Making Test [34, 38], Auditory Odd Ball [40], Modified Attention Network Task [42], Auditory Continuous Performance Test [32], and COMPASS [33].

From the various forms of fruits and vegetables and cognitive tests, research has shown inconsistent results. While some studies showed a significant impact of the effectiveness of fruits and vegetables on attention [31, 32, 38], other studies showed no significant result [37, 40, 42, 43]. In addition, the studies that measured the efficacy of memory also showed mixed results, whereby the studies from several studies [34,35,36, 39, 42] showed a significant result, whereas studies from others did not [33, 37, 38]. Because of the conflicting results in studies, this systematic review aims to synthesis existing research of the effects of fruit and vegetable intake on memory and attention in randomized controlled trials in different age groups.

Materials and methods

This study was registered at PROSPERO (ID: CRD42022308658). It was conducted according to the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analyses (PRISMA) guidelines (refer to Fig. 2). This study only involved secondary data retrieval and analysis, so no ethical approval was needed or sought.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Prima Flowchart

Literature search

Two investigators (KKL and VK) independently searched EBSCOhost, ProQuest, PubMed, and Web of Science on 06/09/2022. The following search terms were used: (fruit OR vegetable OR plant capsule) AND (selective attention OR divided attention OR sustained attention OR attention) AND (memory OR sensory memory OR short-term memory OR long-term memory OR working memory OR episodic memory OR semantic memory OR procedural memory OR autobiographical memory OR iconic memory OR echoic memory OR semantic memory OR declarative memory) AND (intervention OR randomized OR RCT or placebo* OR clinical trial) (refer to Appendix 1).

All searches were conducted by EndNote 20, and duplicated articles were removed. Then, titles and abstracts were screened to identify relevant articles to be included. Finally, articles with full texts were assessed for eligibility in this systematic review.

Inclusion criteria and exclusion criteria

Any experimental studies that reported the consumption of any types of fruits and vegetables with the outcome measures of memory and attention that fulfilled the inclusion criteria were analysed. The inclusion criteria were peer-reviewed articles; randomized controlled trials; studies that included any fruits or vegetables either in the form of capsules, powder, or fresh fruits or vegetables; and outcome measures that were memory and attention scores. The exclusion criteria were conference papers, proposals, and outcome measures that did not have the mean score for pre- and post-intervention.

Study selection

All relevant articles meeting the inclusion criteria were imported into Endnote software, version 20. Identifying duplication was performed first. In the study selection process, two investigators (KKL and VK) screened the titles and abstracts and then filtered out the full-text articles that did not meet the inclusion criteria. At this stage, disagreements primarily arose, because of uncertainties on the abstract as to whether it actually fulfilled our inclusion criteria. For example, articles involving Mediterranean diet were extracted during the selection phase. But these studies involving the Mediterranean diet emphasized not only plant-based foods such as fruits and vegetables but also on, nuts, whole grains, etc. Hence, during the review process, it became apparent that articles discussing special diet plans, which did not exclusively focus on fruits and vegetables, had to be excluded. To address these discrepancies, any disagreement between investigators was resolved through discussions with CYC, OPB and TKW before reaching a final decision.

Data extraction

Data such as the last name of the first author, year of publication, country, objectives of the study, study design, intervention duration, sample size, gender, age, health status, and measures of the study were extracted from the articles. In addition, the outcome measures included data on memory and attention, with baseline means and standard deviation, final means and standard deviation, and the p value for the difference in mean change between the intervention and control groups. Two investigators (KKL and VK) extracted the data independently, and any differences were resolved between them. An example of a difference was the variation in the reporting of cognitive function. Specifically, some articles presented results as the number of correct answers, while others expressed it as the percentage of mean changes. To address these discrepancies, a thorough discussion was conducted and the final decision was based on a consensus between the two investigators.

Quality assessment

The two investigators (KKL and VK) used the Revised Cochrane risk-of-bias tool for randomized trials (RoB2) to assess the risk of bias for each study independently [44]. All sources of bias (randomization process, deviation from intended intervention, missing outcome data, measurement of the outcome, selection of the report result) were evaluated accordingly. Any differences were discussed and resolved by the investigators.

Results

Description of included studies

The literature search and selection processes are presented in Fig. 2 in the appendix. From the literature search, 81 articles were identified from databases in the initial screening. After removing 42 duplicate articles, 39 articles were identified for review of the title and abstract. After screening the title and abstract, 17 articles (such as studies focusing on medicinal plants or review papers) did not fulfil the objective of our study and were excluded. The remaining 22 articles that fulfilled our criteria were further assessed by reading the full text of the articles. With the evaluation of full-text articles, only 13 articles were eligible for quantitative analysis, while nine were excluded because the studies did not include relevant data, such as pretest results (four articles), was not a randomized controlled trial (one article), without a suitable comparator (one article), not a human study (one article), or conference papers (two articles).

Characteristics of included studies

Thirteen [13] articles were finally included in this systematic review (Table 1). As these articles used different memory and attention measuring tools for their baseline and outcome measures, they are not suitable for meta-analysis.

Quality assessment

The assessment of risk of bias is summarized in Fig. 3. Most of the studies have described the randomization process and allocation of intervention or placebo with sufficient details. Thus, they were judged as having a low risk of bias within this domain. Only one study had one concern—the study design was not clear if it was a single-blinded or double-blinded study [43]. However, the study was still included because it still provided data on the efficacy of fruit and vegetable intake consumption on attention and memory. Furthermore, this study was a crossover design where all the participants participated in all the arms.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Assessment of risk of bias

Participants

There was a total of 472 participants from the 13 articles, with 179 (37.9%) males and 293 (62.1%) females. The ages of all participants ranged from 6 to 80 years. Nine studies were on healthy participants [31, 33, 36, 37, 39,40,41,42,43], one study on participants with mild-moderate dementia [34], one article on participants with early memory decline [35], one article on participants with poor cognitive function assessed by Rey’s Auditory Verbal Learning Test [38], and one article that did not report the health condition of the participants [32] ( Table 1).

Table 1 Characteristics of the included studies

All 13 trials included Western or Asian participants. Six trials were conducted in Western countries such as Spain [31], Australia [34], and the UK [33, 36, 37, 41]; three trials were conducted in Asia such as Korea [32], Malaysia [38], and Thailand [40]; and four trials did not report the country of research [35, 39, 42, 43].

Supplementation: forms of fruits and vegetables, duration, and outcomes

Forms of fruits and vegetables

Four major forms of fruit and vegetable intake were used in these 13 studies: powder form made from fruits and vegetable extract, capsule, frozen fruits, and juices made from fresh fruits/vegetables.

Different types of fruits were extracted and reproduced in powder form. For example, blueberry powder was used by Miller et al. [39], Whyte et al. [41] and Dodd et al. [43], and mulberry powder was used by Thukham-Mee [40]. Supplementation with blueberry powder showed a significant improvement in short-term memory in the research of Miller et al. [39] and Whyte et al. [41], but there were no significant changes in the research by Dodd and colleagues (2019). Furthermore, Miller et al., Whyte et al. [41], and Dodd et al. [43] showed that there was no significant improvement in long-term memory. On the other hand, mulberry powder supplementation by Thukham-Mee [40] showed no significant improvement in either attention or memory. These studies of fruit-derived powder had yielded mixed results on changes in cognitive function. However, it is noteworthy that the duration and dosage of these supplementation varied across the studies included in our systematic review. This variation in duration and dose may contribute to the differences in cognitive outcome. The specific details regarding the duration and dosage will be discussed in later section below.

In terms of the use of capsules, Chung et al. [32] extracted Lycium Chinese fruits into capsule form, and Carrillo et al. [31] distributed capsules with a mixture of dehydrated fruit and vegetable. Both studies showed a significant improvement in both attention and working memory.

Only one study conducted by Barfoot et al. [42] used freeze-dried blueberries and blended them with milk. However, there was no significant improvement in either attention or memory in this study.

Different types of juices were used such as cherry juice [34], grape juice [33, 35, 36], commercial juices with high flavonoid content [37], and mixed juices [38]. The research using juices had mixed results. Cherry juice showed significant improvement in short-term memory and long-term memory but no significant changes in attention and semantic memory [34].

Grape juice supplementation showed a significant improvement in long-term memory, short-term memory, and spatial memory [36]. However, the results on total memory recall were mixed, where Krikorian et al. [35]showed a significant improvement, but Haskell-Ramsay et al. [33] showed no significant improvement.

Commercial juice did not show any significant changes in attention and memory [37]. On the other hand, mixed juiced supplementation only showed significant changes in attention when measured by the Comprehensive Trail Making Test (CTMT) Trail 1, but not CTMT Trail 2 to 5 and CTMT composite index [38]. Trail 1 is where participants connect all the numbers orderly; trail 2 is where participants are requested to connect all numbers orderly with empty circles as distractors; trail 3 is where participants have to connect all numbers ordinarily with empty circles and patterned circles as distractors; trail 4 is to connect numbers orderly where the numbers are presented in Arabic or letters; and trail 5 is where the participant has to connect the number and letter A to L orderly in an alternating sequence, for example, 1-A-2-B-3-C [45]. This result suggests that mixed juice supplementation had a positive effect on attention only and may not extend to more complex tasks involving distractors or demonstrate cognitive flexibility.

Duration

There were two different intervention durations: acute effects and short-term effects. Acute effects were defined as outcomes that were measured immediately or up to 6 hours post-intervention [41, 42, 46]; the short-term effect was defined as when the outcome was measured after a short intervention period, ranging from 10 to 12 weeks post-intervention [47, 48].

On the acute effect, one study measured the outcome 20 min after the supplementation [33], two study outcomes were measured 2 hours after the supplementation[37, 43], one study outcome was measured 3 hours after the supplementation [40], and two study outcomes were measured 6 hours after the supplementation [41, 42]. On short-term effects, one study measured the outcome after 10 weeks of intervention [38], one study was 11 weeks [32], three studies were 12 weeks [34,35,36], one study was approximately 13 weeks (which is 90 days) [39], and one study was 16 weeks [31]. Upon reviewing the results from these articles, the majority showed that supplementation demonstrated a significant effect on attention up to 6 h (i.e. the acute effect) post supplementation [33]. Similarly, majority of the studies also showed that supplementation had a significant short-term effect on memory and attention periods of across studies [31,32,33,34,35,36, 36, 38,39,40,41,42].

Cognitive measurements and outcomes

The outcome measures in all the studies are summarized in Table 2, and the quatitative analysis is summarized in Tables 3 and 4.There was a total of 24 types of cognitive tests in the 13 articles. The domains of cognitive tests in these studies included attention and memory [31,32,33,34,35,36,37,38,39,40,41,42,43].

Table 2 Description of cognitive measurements
Table 3 Description of the effect of intervention on attention
Table 4 Description of the effect of intervention on memory

Attention

In this review, eleven studies measured attention using seven different cognitive tests. Each test assessed a different aspect of attention.

The selective attention tests, including Stroop Test, Frankfurt Attention Inventory (FAIR), and Modified Attention Network Task (MANT), were employed. The Stroop Test was used by multiple studies and measured by Carrillo et al. [31] after 16 weeks of dehydrated fruits and vegetables capsules supplementation, Dodd et al. [43] after 2 hours of freeze-dried blueberry supplementation, Thukham-Mee [40] after 3 hours of mulberry powder supplementation, and Lamport et al. [37] after 2 hours of fruit juice supplementation. Stroop Test assesses the individual’s ability to focus on a relevant stimulus (colour of the words shown) while ignoring irrelevant information (meaning of the word) [49]. The results only showed significant improvement in selective attention with short-term supplementation, and this improvement was not observed in the immediate measurement of attention after the supplementation [31]. However, the result has to be carefully interpreted as these studies have different supplementation which may exhibit different outcomes. Additionally, a recent study has questioned the reliability of the Stroop Test in studying attention [50]; thus, careful consideration must be taken when choosing the selective attention test.

FAIR was used by Chung et al. [32], and this test shares a similar pattern with the Stroop Test, but the FAIR test is quicker to do, and it also involves identifying the stimulus while ignoring the distractors [51]. Chung et al. [32] measured the outcome after 11 weeks of Lycium supplementation, and the results showed significant improvement.

MANT is used by Barfoot et al. [42] where participants were required to respond to the relevant stimuli (the direction of a single arrow appearing opposite to many other arrows) and ignore the distractors. In addition, MANT involves not only selective attention, but also orientation, alertness, and executive control [52]. The outcome was measured 2–6 hours after freeze-dried blueberry supplementation, and the results did not show any significant improvement.

Rosli et al. [38] utilized the Comprehensive Trail Making Test (CTMT), a measure of divided attention [53]. Divided attention is the cognitive ability to focus on multiple tasks simultaneously. CTMT is derived from Trail Making Test (TMT) and consists of two additional tasks compared with TMT to make up a total of five tasks. CTMT overcomes the shortcomings of TMT while increasing the difficulty and complexity of the tasks [53]. The task requires participants to shift between numbers and letters simultaneously. However, it is noteworthy that TMT lacks specificity in discriminating executive function from working memory [54,55,56]. Rosli et al. [38] measured the divided attention after 10 weeks of juice extract supplementation, and the result showed significant improvement.

Attention is a complex process, and attention tests such as Auditory Continuous Performance Test, and Go-Nogo Task used in the reviewed studies assess multiple of attention.

Chung et al. [32] utilized Auditory Continuous Performance Test, measuring sustained attention and selective attention over auditory stimuli [32, 57]. This test examines the ability to maintain focus on auditory stimulus over a long period of time, without being distracted. The outcome was measured after 11 weeks of Lycium capsule supplementation, and the result showed significant improvement.

Lamport et al. [37] utilized the Go-Nogo Task to measure the sustained attention and inhibition where the task required participants to maintain their focus and respond to relevant stimuli (Go) and inhibit the irrelevant stimuli (Nogo) [37]. The cognitive function was measured after 2 hours of fruit juice supplementation, and the result did not show any significant improvement.

Haskell-Ramsay et al. [33] used Computerized Mental Performance Assessment System (COMPASS). COMPASS is a computerized test with a cluster of cognitive tests which includes assessing memory and attention. The attention tests included the attention aspect of vigilance and sustained attention. Participants are required to maintain their focus for an extended period of time and respond to the stimuli when the stimuli appear. The attention tests were measured after 20 min of grape juice supplementation, and the results showed a significant improvement in the reaction time, but not in accuracy.

Based on the different cognitive tests measuring attention in this review, we observed a significant improvement in selective attention, sustained attention, and divided attention with supplementation of fruit and vegetable intake, particularly in the short-term studies ranging from 11 to 16 weeks [31, 32, 38]. However, when examining the acute effects, where measurements were conducted 2 to 3 hours after the supplementation, no significant improvement was found [37, 40, 43]. In light of these findings, it is essential to be mindful of the timing of the post-supplementation measurement.

Memory

Different aspects of memory were assessed in the various studies, for example, memory was evaluated as composite memory, short-term memory, long-term memory, working memory, immediate and delay memory, semantic memory, and spatial memory.

Composite memory was measured by the combination of various memory domains, for example, the Computerized Mental Performance Assessment System (COMPASS) and California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT).

Haskell-Ramsay et al. [33] utilized COMPASS which combines episodic memory and working memory to evaluate memory function as whole. Krikorian et al. [35] used CVLT to measure the composite score of short-term and long-term memory recall. CVLT is a measure that is derived from the Auditory Verbal Learning Test, and it includes different cognitive measures such as short-term memory recall and recognition, long-term memory recall and recognition, and learning strategies.

Krikorian [35], who used CVLT to test memory recall, showed a significant improvement after 12 weeks of grape juice supplementation in the supplementation group compared to placebo. However, the study by Haskell-Ramsay et al. [33] did not show any significant improvement after 20 min of freeze-dried blueberry supplementation. Although both Krikorian [35] and Haskell-Ramsay et al. [33] showed contradictory results in composite memory, it could have been because of the difference in length of the supplementation where it was much longer (12 weeks) by Krikorian [35] compared with a much shorter period of supplementation Haskell-Ramsay et al. [33].

Eight studies measured short-term memory recall by using the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT), Visual Verbal Learning Test (VVLT), Letter Memory, Logical Memory, Immediate Recall, Global Cognitive Function, and Digit Span Task [34, 36,37,38,39, 41,42,43].

Three studies used the RAVLT [34, 38, 42], and one study used the modified RAVLT [41] to test short-term memory. The results were mixed with the RAVLT test on short-term memory recall with different durations of intervention. Barfoot et al. [42], Kent et al. [34], and Whyte et al. [41] showed a significant improvement in attention when the RAVLT test was administered at 6 hours [41, 42] and 12 weeks [34] of consumption of the supplement. However, the result from Rosli and colleagues (2021), who had administered the RAVLT after 10 weeks of supplementation consumption, did not show any significant improvement in short-term memory.

Lamport et al. [36] utilized VVLT to measure short-term memory recall. VVLT serves as a visual analogue of the RAVLT. The result was a significant improvement of short-term memory recall after 12 weeks of grape juice supplementation.

Lamport et al. [37] utilized multiple memory tests which included Letter Memroy, Logical Memory (which is adapted from Wechsler Memory scale [36] which originally measured verbal episodic memory including immediate recall, delayed recall, and delay recognition [58]). However, the results did not show any significant effect on the any of the memory tests after 2 hours of fruit juice supplementation.

Dodd et al. [43] used a global cognitive function test which consists 14 tasks assessing different cognitive domains (memory, attention, executive function). This global cognitive function test also includes short-term memory. Results did not show any significant improvement after 2 hours of freeze-dried blueberry supplementation.

Miller et al. [39] used the Digit Span Task to measure short-term memory with immediate recall and short-delay recall in older adults. The results showed no significant improvement in short-term memory recall after approximately 13 weeks of blueberry powder supplementation.

Another aspect of memory that was found in this review is long-term memory recall. Long-term memory recall was measured in seven studies with RAVLT, CVLT, VVLT, Delayed Word Recall, and Global Cognitive Function [34, 36,37,38,39, 42, 43]. Out of these seven studies, only Kent et al. [34] in a 12-week intervention study showed a significant improvement measured by CVLT. Furthermore, Lamport et al. [36] also showed a significant improvement with a 12-week intervention measured by VVLT, while the rest of the studies did not show significant changes.

With reference to memory, there were studies which measured immediate and delay recognition. Immediate and delayed recognition was measured by Dodd et al. [43] using Global Cognitive Function. However, the result was not significant after 2 hours of freeze-dried blueberry supplementation.

Working memory was also evaluated in the studies of this review. Working memory was measured in five studies with different cognitive task such as Digit Span Test [32, 38], Self-ordered Pointing Task, Reynold Intellectual Screening Test (RIST) [31], and Computerized Battery Test designed by Thukham-Mee [40] consisting 10 memory tasks. RIST is a brief screening measure of intelligence [59], but Carrillo [31] used it to evaluate working memory.

The results showed a significant improvement after 16 weeks of intervention using RIST [31], similar to Chung’s et al. [32] study, whose outcome was measured by Digit Span Task after 11 weeks of intervention. However, Rosli [38], who also used the Digit Span Task with 10 weeks of intervention, did not show any significant improvement after the intervention. These two studies presented contradictory results due probably to different supplements consisting of different micronutrients and different durations. Chung et al. [32] used Lycium, which contained 3.41 mg/g betaine, for 11 weeks, while Rosli [38] used a mixture of pomegranate with guava and roselle extract, which contained 609 mg/100 ml phenolic, for 10 weeks.

Furthermore, Thukham-Mee et al. [40] measured working memory outcomes after 3 hours of supplementation using a computerized battery test, and Kent et al. [34], who conducted a 12-week supplementation and measured the outcome using the Self-ordered Pointing Task, did not report any significant differences in working memory.

Another aspect of memory is spatial memory which refers to ability to remember the location, physical arrangement of objects, and features of the environment [60]. Spatial memory was measured in five studies by using the Visual Spatial Learning Test (VSLT), Visual Spatial Learning Test (VSLT), Spatial Paired Associate Learning Test, and Spatial Delayed Recall Test [35,36,37]. Among all these different measurements, only Lamport et al. [36] showed a significant improvement in spatial memory with 12 weeks of grape juice supplementation using the VSLT.

The last aspect of the memory in this review is sematic memory which refers to general knowledge about the world [61]. Semantic memory was measured by Kent et al. [34] using the Boston Naming Test. However, the result was not significant after 12 weeks of cherry juice supplementation.

Discussion

This systematic review summarizes 13 studies that examined the effect of fruit and vegetable consumption on cognition. In general, out of 13 studies, 55.3% of longitudinal studies showed a significant improvement in memory and attention for the supplementation group compared with the placebo group. However, only 8.8% studies on acute effects showed significant improvement. The divergent outcome on effect between longitudinal and acute supplementation prompts the need for exploration into the temporal dynamics of the effects of fruit and vegetable intake on cognitive function. Previous research using polyphenols, derived from fruits and vegetables, showed accumulation in the hippocampus [62], an important region for memory functioning [63] and for increasing neurogenesis [64]. In addition, this result is aligned to a 25-year prospective study which showed that vegetable intake is associated with better cognitive performance [65]. Thus, a longer duration of supplementation may be needed to modulate brain function. Based on our reviews, a few studies using polyphenols also reported significant improvement in memory [31, 36].

Different supplements were used in these 13 studies. Four out of these 13 studies used blueberries as an intervention to test the effect on attention and memory [39, 41,42,43]. Blueberries were dispensed in the form of freeze-dried blueberries [42, 43] and blueberry powder [39, 41]. Immediate memory recall may be improved with longer-term consumption of blueberries. Based on our review, consumption of blueberry powder for a longer duration of 12 weeks showed significant improvement in immediate memory recall measured by CVLT for older adults [39] compared with other studies that measured the effect after 2 hours of consumption [43]. However, studies on children’s immediate memory recall 6 hours after consumption of blueberry powder or fresh blueberries showed a significant improvement as measured by the RVLT [41, 42]. This suggests that a longer duration of consumption is needed for a significant improvement in immediate memory for older adults, while in children, short-term consumption seems to work positively. These results raised concerns of the underlying physiological factor of different age groups on cognitive function. Considering the physiological changes associated with aging, older adults experience reduction in digestive enzymes [66], potentially impacting the breakdown of molecular bonds and absorption of nutrients [67]. In general, 65% of polyphenols are released in the stomach, and 10% are released in the intestine [68]. However, bioavailability of polyphenols by older adults may be influenced by the efficiency of the digestive system. Consequently, smaller amounts of micronutrients are absorbed by older adults compared with children, perhaps explaining the improved memory recall with a longer period of supplementation as it is needed before the nutrients can show an effect. In addition, this finding aligns with the concept of STAC, suggesting that compensatory intervention such as a change of dietary habits can play a crucial role in mitigating cognitive decline associated with aging.

Another supplement used by many researchers was grapes [33, 35, 36]. Grape juice is beneficial for memory with the consumption of 12 weeks based on measurements of CVLT and VSLT [35, 36]. Furthermore, a higher dose of grape juice (621 ml) per day for 12 weeks is needed in the older adult population to significantly improve memory [35] than in the younger adult population, where only 350 ml was needed to show a significant change in memory [36]. This again suggests and supports that either higher doses of supplementation or longer duration (which effectively results in higher total amount of supplementation) are needed in older adults for it to have a beneficial effect.

Both blueberries and grapes have a positive effect on cognitive function, which could be explained by biological mechanisms. Blueberries and grapes have a high polyphenol content, which serves as an antioxidant [69], slowing oxidative stress [70, 71] and thus reducing oxidative damage [8, 9, 72]. In addition, pterostilbene (PTS), one of the polyphenols, found in blueberries and grapes has been shown to have various other effects, such as antioxidant, antiinflammation, and anticancer [73, 74]. With its benefits, it may serve as a protective factor against cognitive deterioration and neurodegenerative diseases such as Alzheimer’s disease and other dementias.

The nutritional value of fruits and vegetables can be affected by many factors, including the method of processing and storage. For example, microbial contamination of goji berries varies depending on the country of cultivation and its preservation methods [75]. To overcome and preserve the degradation of nutrients, studies using capsule supplementation have been done. Researchers extracted dehydrated fruit and vegetable [31] or dried Lycium Chinese fruits [32] and repackaged them into capsule form. Both studies which administered the supplement in capsule form, showed a significant improvement in memory and attention. The encapsulation of supplement protects the bioactive compounds from oxidation and degradation due to light exposure [76] or processing temperature [77]. However, it also requires professional skills to reproduce the capsules, and this is done at a higher production cost.

Strengths and limitations

This review summarizes the effect of consumption of fruit and vegetable intake on memory and attention from 13 articles. The review focuses on randomized control trials that minimized other confounding variables, and it indicates that fruit and vegetable consumption based on several experimental studies have positive effects on cognitive functions.

However, there are several limitations to this review. A small number of available studies with different forms of fruit and vegetable intake with different micronutrient contents and different outcome measures have made it difficult to draw a definitive conclusion on the dose/quantity/duration that are needed to achieve a significant improvement in memory and attention. In addition, 26 different cognitive tests were used to measure cognitive function in these studies. It is difficult to draw conclusions on the sensitivity of different cognitive tests due to a lack of comparison using the same test.

Conclusion

The studies included in this systematic review highlighted the effects of fruit and vegetable intake on memory and attention. This systematic review showed that fruit and vegetable intake consumption significantly improved both attention and memory when the consumption lasted for 10 to 12 weeks. There are different forms of fruit consumption, such as fresh juice, powder extract, or capsule. Children who consumed blueberries showed improvement on the immediate recall test. In addition, older adults have better memory recall with a longer consumption of 12 weeks of blueberry powder consumption by using the CLVLT, and they need a higher dosage of consumption. In conclusion, this systematic review showed that fruit and vegetable intake consumption tended to improve memory and attention. Hence, awareness of the benefit of fruit and vegetable intake consumption is important and should be encouraged to maintain cognitive health.

Availability of data and materials

The data supporting this systematic review are from previously reported studies and datasets, which have been cited. The processed data are available from the corresponding author upon request.

References

  1. Alissa EM, Ferns GA. Dietary fruits and vegetables and cardiovascular diseases risk. Crit Rev Food Sci Nutr. 2015;57(9):1950–62.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Pem D, Jeewon R. Fruit and vegetable intake: benefits and progress of nutrition education interventions-narrative review article. Iran J Public Health. 2015;44(10):1309–21.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Slavin JL, Lloyd B. Health benefits of fruits and vegetables. Adv Nutr. 2012;3:506–16.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Calfio C, Gonzalez A, Singh SK, Rojo LE, MacCioni RB. The emerging role of nutraceuticals and phytochemicals in the prevention and treatment of Alzheimer’s disease. J Alzheimers Dis. 2020;77(1):33–51.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Howes MJR, Perry NSL, Vásquez-Londoño C, Perry EK. Role of phytochemicals as nutraceuticals for cognitive functions affected in ageing. Br J Pharmacol. 2020;177(6):1294–315.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Mocan A, Cairone F, Locatelli M, Cacciagrano F, Carradori S, Vodnar DC, et al. Polyphenols from lycium barbarum (Goji) fruit European cultivars at different maturation steps: extraction, hplc-dad analyses, and biological evaluation. Antioxidants. 2019;8(11):1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Taverniti V, Fracassetti D, Del Bo’ C, Lanti C, Minuzzo M, Klimis-Zacas D, et al. Immunomodulatory effect of a wild blueberry anthocyanin-rich extract in human caco-2 intestinal cells. J Agric Food Chem. 2014;62(33):8346–51.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Wu T, Lv H, Wang F, Wang Y. Characterization of polyphenols from Lycium ruthenicum fruit by UPLC-Q-TOF/MSE and their antioxidant activity in Caco-2 cells. J Agric Food Chem. 2016;64(11):2280–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Zhao D, Simon JE, Wu Q. A critical review on grape polyphenols for neuroprotection: strategies to enhance bioefficacy. Crit Rev Food Sci Nutr. 2020;60(4):597–625.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Ganesan K, Xu B. A critical review on polyphenols and health benefits of black soybeans. Nutrients. 2017;9(5):1–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Cicero AFG, Fogacci F, Banach M. Botanicals and phytochemicals active on cognitive decline: the clinical evidence. Pharmacol Res. 2018;1(130):204–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. WHO. Risk reduction of cognitive decline and dementia: WHO guidelines. Who. Geneva; 2019. 96 p.

  13. Smits LL, Van Harten AC, Pijnenburg YAL, Koedam ELGE, Bouwman FH, Sistermans N, et al. Trajectories of cognitive decline in different types of dementia. Psychol Med. 2015;45(5):1051–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Parikh PK, Troyer AK, Maione AM, Murphy KJ. The impact of memory change on daily life in normal aging and mild cognitive impairment. Gerontologist. 2016;56(5):877–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Goldman JG, Vernaleo BA, Camicioli R, Dahodwala N, Dobkin RD, Ellis T, et al. Cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease: a report from a multidisciplinary symposium on unmet needs and future directions to maintain cognitive health. Npj Park Dis. 2018;4(1):1–11.

    Google Scholar 

  16. Salthouse T. Consequences of age-related cognitive declines. Annu Rev Psychol. 2012;63(1):201–26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Alzoubi KH, Mayyas FA, Mahafzah R, Khabour OF. Melatonin prevents memory impairment induced by high-fat diet: role of oxidative stress. Behav Brain Res. 2018;336:93–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Kandlur A, Satyamoorthy K, Gangadharan G. Oxidative Stress in Cognitive and Epigenetic Aging: A Retrospective Glance. Front Mol Neurosci. 2020;13:41.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. JW Kalat 2009 Biological Psychology 242 13 Cengage Learning Science. United States of America

  20. Posner MI, Petersen SE. The attention system of the human brain. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1990;13:25–42.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Schmitz TW, Duncan J. Normalization and the cholinergic microcircuit: a unified basis for attention. Trends Cogn Sci. 2018;22(5):422–37.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Lowet E, Gomes B, Srinivasan K, Zhou H, Schafer RJ, Desimone R. Enhanced neural processing by covert attention only during microsaccades directed toward the attended stimulus. Neuron. 2018;99(1):207-214.e3.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Cercignani M, Giulietti G, Dowell NG, Gabel M, Broad R, Leigh PN, et al. Characterizing axonal myelination within the healthy population: a tract-by-tract mapping of effects of age and gender on the fiber g-ratio. Neurobiol Aging. 2017;49:109–18.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Marin MA, Carmichael ST. Mechanisms of demyelination and remyelination in the young and aged brain following white matter stroke. Neurobiol Dis. 2019;126:5–12.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Park DC, Reuter-Lorenz P. The adaptive brain: aging and neurocognitive scaffolding. Annu Rev Psychol. 2009;60:173–96.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Reuter-Lorenz PA, Park DC. How does it STAC up? Revisiting the scaffolding theory of aging and cognition. Neuropsychol Rev. 2014;24(3):355–70.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Goh JO, Park DC. Neuroplasticity and cognitive aging: the scaffolding theory of aging and cognition. Restor Neurol Neurosci. 2009;27(5):391–403.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Painter K. Diet still holds the best hope for feeding your brain. The Post - Crescent. 2015;20:3.

    Google Scholar 

  29. Loef M, Walach H. Fruit, vegetables and prevention of cognitive decline or dementia: a systematic review of cohort studies. J Nutr Health Aging. 2012;16(7):626–30.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Carrillo JÁ, Zafrilla MP, Marhuenda J. Cognitive function and consumption of fruit and vegetable polyphenols in a young population: is there a relationship? Foods. 2019;8(10):507.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Carrillo JÁ, Arcusa R, Zafrilla MP, Marhuenda J. Effects of fruit and vegetable-based nutraceutical on cognitive function in a healthy population: placebo-controlled, double-blind, and randomized clinical trial. Antioxidants. 2021;10(1):1–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Chung SY, Kang M, Hong SB, Bae H, Cho SHSH. Standardized lycium chinense fruit extract enhances attention and cognitive function in healthy young people by a double-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled, crossover trial. J Res Med Sci Publ Wolters Kluwer-Medknow. 2019;(1).

  33. Haskell-Ramsay CF, Stuart RC, Okello EJ, Watson AW. Cognitive and mood improvements following acute supplementation with purple grape juice in healthy young adults. Eur J Nutr. 2017;56(8):2621–31.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Kent K, Charlton K, Roodenrys S, Batterham M, Potter J, Traynor V, et al. Consumption of anthocyanin-rich cherry juice for 12 weeks improves memory and cognition in older adults with mild-to-moderate dementia. Eur J Nutr. 2015;56(1):333–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Krikorian R, Nash TA, Shidler MD, Shukitt-Hale B, Joseph JA. Concord grape juice supplementation improves memory function in older adults with mild cognitive impairment. Br J Nutr. 2009;103(5):730–4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Lamport DJ, Lawton CL, Merat N, Jamson H, Myrissa K, Hofman D, et al. Concord grape juice, cognitive function, and driving performance: a 12-wk, placebo-controlled, randomized crossover trial in mothers of preteen children. Am J Clin Nutr. 2016;103(3):775–83.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Lamport DJ, Pal D, Macready AL, Barbosa-Boucas S, Fletcher JM, Williams CM, et al. The effects of flavanone-rich citrus juice on cognitive function and cerebral blood flow: an acute, randomised, placebo-controlled cross-over trial in healthy, young adults. Br J Nutr. 2016;116(12):2160–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Rosli H, Shahar S, Rajab NF, Che Din N, Haron H. The effects of polyphenols-rich tropical fruit juice on cognitive function and metabolomics profile: a randomized controlled trial in middle-aged women. Nutr Neurosci. 2021;0(0):1–17.

  39. Miller MG, Hamilton DA, Joseph JA, Shukitt-Hale B. Dietary blueberry improves cognition among older adults in a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. Eur J Nutr. 2017;57(3):1169–80.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Thukham-Mee W, Wattanathorn J, Kirisattayakul W, Wannanon P. Effect of single administration of mulberry milk on the cognitive function of 6–12-year-old children: Results from a randomized, placebo-controlled, crossover study. Oxid Med Cell Longev. 2020;2020.

  41. Whyte AR, Schafer G, Williams CM. Cognitive effects following acute wild blueberry supplementation in 7- to 10-year-old children. Eur J Nutr. 2015;55(6):2151–62.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Barfoot KL, May G, Lamport DJ, Ricketts J, Riddell PM, Williams CM. The effects of acute wild blueberry supplementation on the cognition of 7–10-year-old schoolchildren. Eur J Nutr. 2019;58(7):2911–20.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Dodd GF, Williams CM, Butler LT, Spencer JPE. Acute effects of flavonoid-rich blueberry on cognitive and vascular function in healthy older adults. Nutr Healthy Aging. 2019;5(2):119–32.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  44. Sterne JAC, Savović J, Page MJ, Elbers RG, Blencowe NS, Boutron I, et al. RoB 2: a revised tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials. BMJ [Internet]. 2019 Aug 28;366. Available from: https://www.bmj.com/content/366/bmj.l4898 [cited 2021 Dec 27]

  45. Reynolds CR, Vannest KJ, Fletcher-Janzen E. Encyclopedia of special education: a reference for the education of children, adolescents, and adults with disabilities and other exceptional individuals. Encyclopedia of Special Education. Wiley; 2013. 512–513 p.

  46. George TW, Niwat C, Waroonphan S, Gordon MH, Lovegrove JA. Effects of chronic and acute consumption of fruit-and vegetable-puree-based drinks on vasodilation, risk factors for CVD and the response as a result of the eNOS G298T polymorphism. In: Proceedings of the Nutrition Society. 2009. p. 148–61. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0029665109001165

  47. Arnold C, Jentsch S, Dawczynski J, Böhm V. Age-related macular degeneration: effects of a short-term intervention with an oleaginous kale extract—a pilot study. Nutrition. 2013;29(11–12):1412–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Greenlee H, Gaffney AO, Aycinena AC, Koch P, Contento I, Karmally W, et al. Long-term diet and biomarker changes after a short-term intervention among Hispanic breast cancer survivors: the ¡Cocinar Para Su Salud! randomized controlled trial. Cancer Epidemiol Prev Biomark. 2016;25(11):1491–502.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  49. Upadhayay N, Guragain S, Khadka P. Linguistic Interference in reading Stroop card. Kathmandu Univ Med J. 2022;20(2):178–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Algom D, Fitousi D, Chajut E. Can the Stroop effect serve as the gold standard of conflict monitoring and control? A conceptual critique Mem Cognit. 2022;50(5):883–97.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Ji H, Wu S, Won J, Weng S, Lee S, Seo S, et al. The effects of exergaming on attention in children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: randomized controlled trial. JMIR Serious Games. 2023;9(11): e40438.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Arora S, Lawrence MA, Klein RM. The Attention Network Test Database: ADHD and Cross-Cultural Applications. Front Psychol. 2020;11:388.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  53. Moses J. Comprehensive trail making test (CTMT) test review. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2004;19(5):703–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Linari I, Juantorena GE, Ibáñez A, Petroni A, Kamienkowski JE. Unveiling trail making test: visual and manual trajectories indexing multiple executive processes. Sci Rep. 2022;12(1):14265.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Pérez-Parra JE, Restrepo-de-Mejía F. The trail making test (part B) is associated with working memory: a concurrent validity study. Appl Neuropsychol Adult. 2023;0(0):1–9.

  56. Salthouse TA. What cognitive abilities are involved in trail-making performance? Intelligence. 2011;39(4):222–32.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  57. Tinius TP. The integrated visual and auditory continuous performance test as a neuropsychological measure. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2003;18(5):439–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Ahn YD, Yi D, Joung H, Seo EH, Lee YH, Byun MS, et al. Normative data for the logical memory subtest of the Wechsler Memory Scale-IV in middle-aged and elderly Korean people. Psychiatry Investig. 2019;16(11):793–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  59. Ruchinskas R, Goette W. Reynolds Intellectual Screening Instrument 1st versus 2nd edition in a memory disorder sample. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2021 May 21;36(4):570–7.

  60. Jacobs LF. Memory, Spatial. In: Aminoff MJ, Daroff RB, editors. Encyclopedia of the Neurological Sciences [Internet]. New York: Academic Press; 2003. p. 86–90. Available from: https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B0122268709013757 [cited 2024 Jan 17]

  61. Pitel AL, Eustache F, Beaunieux H. Chapter 13 - Component processes of memory in alcoholism: pattern of compromise and neural substrates. In: Sullivan EV, Pfefferbaum A, editors. Handbook of Clinical Neurology. Elsevier; 2014. p. 211–25. (Alcohol and the Nervous System; vol. 125). Available from: https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780444626196000136 [cited 2024 Jan 17]

  62. Chen TY, Kritchevsky J, Hargett K, Feller K, Klobusnik R, Song BJ, et al. Plasma bioavailability and regional brain distribution of polyphenols from apple/grape seed and bilberry extracts in a young swine model. Mol Nutr Food Res. 2015;59:2432–47.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Vauzour D. Polyphenols and brain health. OCL - Oilseeds Fats Crops Lipids. 2017;24(2):1–7.

    Google Scholar 

  64. Vauzour D. Effect of flavonoids on learning, memory and neurocognitive performance: relevance and potential implications for Alzheimer’s disease pathophysiology. J Sci Food Agric. 2014;94(6):1042–56.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Mao X, Chen C, Xun P, Daviglus ML, Steffen LM, Jacobs DR, et al. Intake of vegetables and fruits through young adulthood is associated with better cognitive function in midlife in the US general population. J Nutr. 2019;149(8):1424–33.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  66. Rémond D, Shahar DR, Gille D, Pinto P, Kachal J, Peyron MA, et al. Understanding the gastrointestinal tract of the elderly to develop dietary solutions that prevent malnutrition. Oncotarget. 2015;6(16):13858.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  67. Terra WR, Ferreira C. Biochemistry and molecular biology of digestion. Insect Mol Biol Biochem. 2012;1:365–418.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Bouayed J, Hoffmann L, Bohn T. Total phenolics, flavonoids, anthocyanins and antioxidant activity following simulated gastro-intestinal digestion and dialysis of apple varieties: bioaccessibility and potential uptake. Food Chem. 2011;128(1):14–21.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Pawlowska E, Szczepanska J, Koskela A, Kaarniranta K, Blasiak J. Dietary polyphenols in age-related macular degeneration: protection against oxidative stress and beyond. Oxid Med Cell Longev. 2019;2019.

  70. Feeney J, Finucane C, Savva GM, Cronin H, Beatty S, Nolan JM, et al. Neurobiology of aging low macular pigment optical density is associated with lower cognitive performance in a large, population-based sample of older adults. Neurobiol Aging. 2013;34(11):2449–56.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Johnson EJ, Vishwanathan R, Johnson MA, Hausman DB, Davey A, Scott TM, et al. Relationship between serum and brain carotenoids, α -tocopherol, and retinol concentrations and cognitive performance in the oldest old from the Georgia centenarian study. J Aging Res. 2013;2013(2013): 951786.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  72. Harasym J, Oledzki R. Effect of fruit and vegetable antioxidants on total antioxidant capacity of blood plasma. Nutrition. 2014;30(5):511–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Nagarajan S, Mohandas S, Ganesan K, Xu B, Ramkumar KM. New insights into dietary pterostilbene: sources, metabolism, and health promotion effects. Molecules. 2022;27(19):6316.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  74. Jayakumar T, Wu MP, Sheu JR, Hsia CW, Bhavan PS, Manubolu M, et al. Involvement of antioxidant defenses and nf-κb/erk signaling in anti-inflammatory effects of pterostilbene, a natural analogue of resveratrol. Appl Sci. 2021;11(10):4666.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  75. Rajkowska K, Otlewska A, Broncel N, Kunicka-Styczyńska A. Microbial diversity and bioactive compounds in dried Lycium barbarum fruits (goji): a comparative study. Molecules. 2023;28(10):4058.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  76. Huvaere K, Skibsted LH. Flavonoids protecting food and beverages against light. J Sci Food Agric. 2015;95(1):20–35.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Phan ADT, Adiamo O, Akter S, Netzel ME, Cozzolino D, Sultanbawa Y. Effects of drying methods and maltodextrin on vitamin C and quality of Terminalia ferdinandiana fruit powder, an emerging Australian functional food ingredient. J Sci Food Agric. 2021;101(12):5132–41.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to express our thanks and gratitude to Sunway University for supporting this work and for their Internal Grant GRTIN-IGS-DMS(S)-24-2022 for funding the research.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

All authors have contributed equally to this manuscript. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Yook-Chin Chia.

Ethics declarations

Ethics and approval and consent to participate

This review contains only studies that complied with required ethical standards. All of the eligible articles included in the systematic review had stated that they had obtained informed consent from their participants.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Khor, K.L., Kumarasuriar, V., Tan, K.W. et al. Effects of fruit and vegetable intake on memory and attention: a systematic review of randomized controlled trials. Syst Rev 13, 151 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13643-024-02547-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13643-024-02547-8

Keywords