Skip to main content

Effects of chronic physical exercise on executive functions and episodic memory in clinical and healthy older adult populations: a systematic review and meta-analysis protocol

Abstract

Background

Executive functions (EFs) and episodic memory are fundamental components of cognition that deteriorate with age and are crucial for independent living. While numerous reviews have explored the effect of exercise on these components in old age, these reviews screened and analyzed selected older adult populations, or specific exercise modes, thus providing only limited answers to the fundamental question on the effect of exercise on cognition in old age. This article describes the protocol for a systematic review and multilevel meta-analytic study aiming at evaluating the effectiveness of different types of chronic exercise in improving and/or maintaining EFs and long-term episodic memory in older adults.

Methods and analysis

The study protocol was written in accordance with the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines. Several databases will be searched. Randomized controlled trials (RCTs) conducted in older adults aged ≥ 60 years providing any kind of planned, structured, and repetitive exercise interventions, and EFs and/or episodic memory measures as outcomes, published in English in peer-reviewed journals and doctoral dissertations will be included. Two independent reviewers will screen the selected articles, while a third reviewer will resolve possible conflicts. The Cochrane risk-of-bias tool will be used to assess the quality of the studies. Finally, data will be extracted from the selected articles, and the formal method of combining individual data from the selected studies will be applied using a random effect multilevel meta-analysis. The data analysis will be conducted with the metafor package in R.

Discussion and conclusion

This review will synthesize the existing evidence and pinpoint gaps existing in the literature on the effects of exercise on EFs and episodic memory in healthy and unhealthy older adults. Findings from this meta-analysis will help to design effective exercise interventions for older adults to improve and/or maintain EFs and episodic memory. Its results will be useful for many researchers and professionals working with older adults and their families.

Systematic review registration

PROSPERO CRD42022367111.

Peer Review reports

Introduction

Background

Developed nations are experiencing unprecedented increases in the population of older adults mostly due to the reduced birth rates and the increased longevity of their citizens. The latest projections by the United Nations suggest that the global population could grow to around 8.5 billion in 2030, 9.7 billion in 2050, and 10.4 billion in 2100 [1]. More importantly, it was estimated that in the European Union, the old-age dependency will increase from 29.6% in 2016 to 51.2% in 2070 [2].

With respect to brain and cognition, aging is the main risk factor for neurodegeneration with prevalence increasing further with age [3]. Given the demographic situation and the relation of aging with cognitive decline, there is great interest in exploring effective ways to improve and/or maintain cognitive functions for independent living [4]. The main approaches to improving brain functionality and cognition in older adults are physical activity (PA), cognitive training, and social engagement [5]. The focus of this paper is PA.

Colcombe and Kramer [6] conducted two decades ago a seminal meta-analytic study on the effect of aerobic fitness on cognition in older adults. The study included 18 intervention studies and showed robust benefits in cognition with the largest fitness-induced benefits occurring for executive control processes, as previously hypothesized by Kramer, Hahn et al. [7]. The magnitude of the effect was moderated by the length of the training intervention, the length of the training sessions, the type of the intervention, aerobic training or aerobic combined with strength training with better results for combined training, and the gender of the participants with larger benefits for women.

The research conducted since then has provided compelling evidence that regular practice of PA can promote and/or maintain cognitive and brain functioning in late adulthood and old age [8,9,10]. The literature usually distinguishes between PA and exercise. The former entails any bodily movement produced by skeletal muscles that increases energy expenditure relative to rest. Exercise is a subcategory of PA that is planned, structured, and repetitive and is more specifically designed to improve one or more components of fitness: cardiorespiratory fitness, flexibility, balance, coordination, strength, and/or power [11].

The main objective of this review focuses on analyzing the effect of various exercise interventions, including aerobic exercise, strength straining, dance, and balance exercises on executive functions (EFs) and episodic memory of older adults. There is agreement among aging researchers that significant declines appear with age in EFs [8, 12] and long-term episodic memory, related to intentional retrieval of episodes [13, 14]; thus, several studies focus on these components [15,16,17,18]. EFs are formed by a series of effortful top-down cognitive processes necessary for mental and physical health, success in life, and cognitive, social, and psychological development [19]. The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) plays a crucial role in the different components of EFs [20] and contributes to these components via functional connectivity with different brain regions [21].

Improvements in fitness are expected to improve EF processes such as coordination, inhibition, planning, and updating of working memory [7] but also cognitive flexibility as well as higher-order executive functions related to reasoning and fluid intelligence. Inhibitory control refers to the ability to control one’s attention and do what is more appropriate in each circumstance. Moreover, inhibitory control allows us to selectively attend to a certain stimulus suppressing other stimuli. Self-control is another aspect of inhibitory control related to resisting temptations and avoiding impulsivity. Inhibitory control declines greatly in normal aging [22], and older adults struggle to avoid distractions [23]. A recent cross-sectional study has showed that the EFs inhibition, shifting, updating, and dual tasking decline in healthy older adults but not with the same intensity with inhibition showing the greatest decline and dual tasking the smallest [24].

Working memory (WM), and more particularly updating of WM, is another key EF that serves to hold verbal or visual-spatial information in mind that is no longer perceptually present and working with it [25]. WM and inhibitory control are closely related and often support one another. The decline in WM with aging correlates with a decrease in the speed of information processing in older adults [26, 27].

The third component of EFs, cognitive flexibility, builds on working memory and inhibitory control. Flexibility means to being able to adjust to changed demands and to change perspectives, task switching, and set shifting. Cognitive flexibility is a property of the cognitive system that helps us to pursue complex tasks [28]. An additional component of EFs is higher-order EFs which is related to reasoning, problem-solving, and planning and is synonymous with fluid intelligence [19].

Episodic memory is a key cognitive process that allows us to represent past experiences and employ these representations to serve current and future goals [29, 30]. It is one of the earliest memory systems that decline with increasing aging. Impaired episodic memory with aging, involving retrieval of personal experiences and their spatial and temporal contexts, is well documented in the literature [31]. At the brain level, the medial temporal lobe and the hippocampus play a crucial role in retrieving information from episodic memory [32].

Since the influential meta-analytic study conducted by Colcombe and Kramer [6], the effect of exercise on EFs and episodic memory has been examined in numerus meta-analyses [33,34,35,36,37,38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47]. However, some reviews included only healthy populations [39, 40, 46], while others included only cognitively impaired or demented older adults [34, 35, 41, 43, 44, 48]. Chen et al. [36] included both healthy and cognitively impaired older adults but not demented. While one review examined only nursing home residents [38], another review [45] included only community-dwelling older adults. On the other site, while one review [33] focused only on aerobic exercise, another review [37] centered merely on resistance training, yet a third one [42] focused on exergames.

The current study addresses the gaps of the existing literature and aims to extend the knowledge of the effect of exercise on the principal components of cognition in old age. Our comprehensive review will potentially include healthy and non-healthy older adults and a wide range of exercise modes. This argument stems from a gap in evidence-based literature as pointed in a recent article [49]. For example, it has been argued that research on older populations is typically biased towards healthy and relatively young older adults, with certain groups of older individuals frequently being excluded from research on aging — especially in studies with physical activity interventions [49]. Such a review will pose a general question on the effect of exercise on cognition in advanced age (a general effect size will be calculated) followed by examining the moderating effect of various exercise modes (e.g., aerobics, strength, balance), several exercise characteristics (e.g., intensity, frequency, length), and a wide range of population characteristics (e.g., education level, percentage of females, health status), protocol characteristics (e.g., type of control group, type of analysis — intention-to-treat vs. per-protocol), and exercise settings (community dwelling and nursing homes). In addition, the present review will make an in-depth examination of the moderating effect of the outcomes used to assess cognitive functions, distinguishing, for example, working memory span indexes (e.g., number of correct responses in reading span tasks) from updating working memory indexes (e.g., error rate in n-back tasks), the latter requiring much more executive control than the former. The choice of adequate indexes of EFs is a very sensitive problem when estimating the effect size of the influence of regular exercise on EFs.

To summarize, the main objective of this systematic review and meta-analysis is to address the gaps encountered in the existing literature and to investigate the advantages of a broad range of exercise interventions on two key cognitive components, EFs and long-term episodic memory, across diverse groups of older adults and considering very selective outcomes. The findings from this review will be instrumental in developing effective training methods to enhance EFs and episodic memory in healthy and unhealthy older adults.

Methods and analysis

The protocol of this review was prepared following the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis Protocols (PRISMA-P) 2015 statement and Cochrane systematic review methodology [50, 51]. The protocol is registered on the International Prospective Register of Systematic Reviews (number CRD42022367111).

Figure 1 presents the planned flow chart of the systematic review and meta-analysis with a summary of the selection process.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Flow chart diagram of the search strategy of the systematic review

Eligibility criteria

Eligibility criteria follows the PICO framework regarding population, intervention, comparator, outcome, and study type.

Population

The study will include participants with a mean age of 60 years or older and a minimum age of 50 years. It will include both healthy older adults and older adults diagnosed with various conditions such as mild cognitive impairment (MCI), Alzheimer’s disease (AD), or Parkinson disease (PD).

Intervention

Any randomized controlled trial (RCT) focusing on the effects of any type of exercise will be screened for inclusion, including aerobic, resistance training, coordination training, and other exercise programs such as tai chi, qigong, dancing, and exergaming. Several main exercise characteristics (type, session duration, session intensity, session frequency, intervention duration) will be assessed.

Comparators

Comparators will include light exercise, stretching, meditation, relaxation, and/or passive control groups (waiting list, treatment as usual, and habitual activities).

Outcomes

Cognitive outcomes will include objectively assessed cognitive domains of EFs (inhibition, working memory, cognitive flexibility, and high-level EFs) and episodic memory. These cognitive domains should have been assessed at baseline and at the end of the intervention through well-validated cognitive tasks and psychological tests. Regarding EFs, the outcomes for assessing inhibitory control may include the Stroop task, Eriksen-Flanker test, Simon task, global–local task, go-no go task, random number generation task, saccade-antisaccade task, and stop-signal task (STT). To assess working memory, the tasks and tests may include the Corsi block-tapping test, reading span task (RST), operation span task (OSpan), backward verbal digit span task, visuospatial N-back task, or tone monitoring task. To assess cognitive flexibility, the instruments may include the Trial Making Test (TMT), the Alternative Uses Task (AUT), Brixton Spatial Anticipation Test (BSAT), Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS, subtests: the Trail Making Test, the Color-Word Test, the Tower Test, the design fluency test, The Sorting Test), Remote Associates Test (RAT), Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP), attentional set-shifting task (AST), or Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST). Among the instruments to assess high-level executive functions are the Raven’s Colored Progressive Matrices (RCPM) and Tower of Hanoi (TOH). In the case of episodic memory, the assessment tools may include the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT), SEMantic Episodic Memory Test (SEMEP), Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS; only the subtests that assess episodic memory), Hopkins Verbal Learning Test (HVLR-R), language-based paradigms, or the 360° video for episodic memory assessment. All the indexes of performance used for each of these tasks will be carefully selected to be sure that they reflect the targeted cognitive function (e.g., interference score in the case of the Stroop task, the Ericksen task, and the Simon task).

Inclusion and exclusion criteria

The inclusion criteria will be age (mean ≥ 60 with a minimum of 50 years), the practice of any type of physical exercise for at least 3 months, and provide outcomes including any EFs or episodic memory measure assessed at baseline (before physical training) and after training (post-training). If there were enough follow-up studies (e.g., 3 months, 6 months after training), they will be analyzed. Characteristics of exercise intervention such as frequency, intensity, type, and/or time of exercise (FITT) of the intervention program will have to be informed. Studies will be excluded if they do not meet the PICO conditions mentioned above, if they are not RCTs, if they do not have at least an active or passive control group, or if the written language is not English.

Research questions

The present study is directed to answer six main research questions described below.

  1. i.

    To what extent does exercise enhance EFs and episodic memory in old age (the global effect)?

  2. ii.

    Do different types of chronic exercise (aerobic, resistance training, coordination training, and other exercise programs, such as Tai Chi or Qigong, dancing, or exergaming) have a different impact on EFs and episodic memory in older adults?

  3. iii.

    Is the type of control group (active vs. passive) a moderator of the changes in the investigated cognitive domains?

  4. iv.

    Is the effect of exercise different in healthy older adults and clinical older adults suffering neuropsychological disorders (MCI, PD, AD)?

  5. v.

    Is age a moderator of the effect of exercise on the investigated components of cognition?

  6. vi.

    Are duration and intensity of exercise moderators of the effect of regular exercise on cognitive aging?

Literature search strategy

An initial search will be conducted at MEDLINE, Embase, PsychINFO, Google Scholar, EBSCO, SportDiscuss, CINAHL, Science Direct Dissertations, Web of Science, and Cochrane Central Registered of Controlled Trials (CENTRAL). These databases were selected because they are the most important and widely used to assure that relevant articles were not missed and in consultation with experienced researchers and librarians. Table 1 shows the detailed search strategy for PsychINFO. In addition, systematic reviews and meta-analyses published on episodic memory and the different EFs processes will be screened to check if the articles included in these publications should be considered in the present review.

Table 1 Search strategy for PsychINFO

Inclusion will be restricted to articles written in English published in peer-reviewed journals and doctoral theses. Studies published in other language will not be included. English is the most widely used scientific language to publish intervention studies and the language used in most systematic reviews and meta-analytic studies. Articles published from the inception will be considered for inclusion. An additional final search in the different databases will be conducted at the end of the review process to include more recently published studies.

After carefully reading all the retrieved articles, the data will be extracted for conducting the meta-analyses.

Data extraction

Once the databases are searched, the retrieved articles will be exported in a Research Information Systems (RIS) format and imported into Rayyan [52], a web application created for article screening. The first step in Rayyan will consist of removing all the duplicates. Then, pairs of reviewers will work independently and blinded on screening articles based on title and abstract. Possible conflicts between the two independent reviewers will be solved by a third reviewer (J. M. R.). After completing the first selection stage by title and abstract, the next step will be retrieving the full articles corresponding to the included articles for careful reading. The idea is to extract in an Excel spreadsheet all the relevant information. The extracted data will include the following: (i) Characteristics: information regarding author(s), journal, publication year, and country; (ii) population: number of participants in each group, participants’ characteristics including mean age, sex, and clinical condition; (iii) interventions: including type of physical activity, intensity, session duration, total duration of the intervention, and adherence; and (iv) outcomes: in terms of tasks and psychological instruments used to assess memory and EFs, including sample size, means, and standard deviations at baseline and post-intervention and other possible time points corresponding (follow-up assessments) to the different (intervention and control) groups.

If a study will be relevant for our analysis but the data necessary to calculate the effect sizes will be missing or just the graphs were available, we will contact the corresponding author by email to ask for the relevant data. If the author does not respond, the missing data will be extracted from the graphs provided in the article using the online tool WebPlotDigitizer version 4.3.

In the case of RCTs with several time points, we will focus on the post-intervention at the end of the physical exercise training. If more time points or follow-up assessments were provided and enough articles contained assessments at 3 or 6 months after the end of the intervention program, the effects will also be considered. We will calculate Hedges’s g as the effect size.

Risk of bias

The risk of bias (RoB) of each included study will be evaluated using the Cochrane ROB 2 tool [50, 53, 54]. Biases are assessed across five areas including randomization, deviations from intended interventions, missing outcome data, outcome measurement, and selection of the reported results. The risk of bias of each study will be assessed based on a series of questions provided for each of the five areas and the possible answers in the following five categories: “yes,” “probably yes,” “no,” “probably no,” and “no information.” Finally, the risk of bias in each area will be assessed as “low risk of bias,” “some concerns,” or “high risk of bias.” Teams of two reviewers will independently assess the risk of bias in the included studies. A third independent reviewer will resolve possible disagreements.

Statistical analysis

Effect sizes (ES) will be modelled using a three-level structure because it is a better approach than a two-level structure when there are several dependent effect sizes in each independent study but only if the heterogeneity of the sampling variance is substantial. In three-level meta-analytic models, three different sources of variance are modelled. The third level represents the variance of effect sizes between studies; the second level describes the variance of effect sizes of the experiments, or measurements nested within each study; and the first level describes the sample variance. In the present study, we will perform a multilevel random-effects analysis using restricted maximum likelihood estimation. This analytical solution was designed to account for the nonindependence among effect sizes. This is the preferred methodology when the sampling variability is not too high. Heterogeneity among effect sizes (I2) will be assessed using the omnibus homogeneity test (Q), 0–40% indicates negligible heterogeneity, 30–60% indicates moderate heterogeneity, and 50–90% suggests substantial heterogeneity. A large Q-value means that differences between effect sizes do not derive from a common population mean from the study samples but are accounted for by other reasons.

The statistical analysis will be performed using rma.mv function of the metaphor package (version 2.4) [55] within the R software environment (version 4.0.1; R Core Team, 2021) [56]. The analytical steps provided by Assink and Wibbelink [57] will be followed. Dot-plot figures will be depicted using Mathematica (version 10.4) with software developed specifically for the present study.

To avoid outliers or influential cases that could distort the results of the meta-analysis, outlier and influential case diagnostics will be performed using the influence function of the metaphor package. The influence function calculates the influence of deleting one case at a time on the model fit or the fitted/residual values. Statistical heterogeneity will be assessed using the I2 test.

After a systematic publication search, it might occur that some studies were missed due to publication bias. That is, intervention studies that did not obtain significant results are not published, either because the authors did not submit them to a journal for publication or because the editor rejected them. We will address this important issue using two complementary statistics. The first explores the relationship between the precision and the observed effect size of the studies (the funnel plot and the statistical test of its asymmetry known as Egger’s regression test) under the assumption that effect sizes drive publication bias. In a funnel plot, the effect sizes are plotted against the standard error. An asymmetric funnel plot would suggest that publication bias exists, for example, an underrepresentation of nonsignificant results and/or negative effects on the bottom left side of the funnel plot. To evaluate the statistical significance of the funnel plots, we will apply the Egger’s test [58]. This test analyzes in a linear regression whether the standardized effect sizes can predict study precision, defined as the inverse of the standard error. The main goal of this analysis is to find a significant regression intercept that differs significantly from zero which would indicate a significant funnel plot asymmetry. We will also use the trim-and-fill method [59, 60] to determine the number of effect sizes that would need to be imputed to restore the symmetry of the funnel plot.

The second statistics we are going to use to assess publication bias is the P-curve technique, which assumes that publication bias is driven primarily through p-values, not by effect sizes. This relatively new methodology is based on the shape of the histogram of p-values, which depends on the sample sizes of studies and the actual effect size of the data. The method determines if the data estimates an actual, non-spurious effect size.

Once we had all the required information regarding the types of interventions, comparators, outcomes, and the healthy or clinical conditions of the participants of the finally included studies, we would be able to provide information regarding search results, descriptive results corresponding to studies and participants’ characteristics, overall effect size, and moderator analyses.

Discussion and conclusion

The demographic data suggest that the world is aging very rapidly, and it is necessary to take actions against the cognitive decline that comes with aging. EFs and episodic memory are fundamental components of cognition that deteriorate with age and are vital for independent living. These cognitive declines significantly impact the performance of activities of daily living, independent living, and well-being among older adults. Previous reviews and meta-analyses screened and analyzed certain older adult populations [39, 46, 34, 41, 48], or specific type of exercise [33, 37, 42], providing limited answer to the question on the effect of exercise on EFs and episodic memory of older adults. The novelty of the present review is that it extends the knowledge about the effects of exercise on specific and central aspects of cognition to include different exercise modes and both healthy and unhealthy older adults.

Considering the key procedures and analyses, this systematic and meta-analytic review follows the PRISMA-P 2015 statement and the Cochrane systematic review methodology [50, 51]. The eligibility criteria of the articles to be included follows the PICO framework (population, intervention, comparator, and outcomes). Articles that met the inclusion criteria will be carefully read by pairs of reviewers who will extract the data for conducting the meta-analysis. Hedges’s g will be calculated as the effect size. Risk of bias of the included studies will be assessed with the Cochrane ROB 2 tool [50, 53, 54] by pairs of reviewers.

If the heterogeneity of the sampling variance is substantial, effect sizes (ES) will be modelled using a three-level structure. This approach is superior than a two-level structure. In a three-level structure, the third level corresponds to the variance of effect sizes between studies, while the second level refers to the effect sizes of the experiments within each study. Finally, the first level describes the sample variance.

The statistical analysis will be conducted using rma.mv function of the metaphor package (version 2.4) within the R software environment (version 4.0.1; R Core Team 2021), following the analytical steps of Assink and Wibbelink [57]. A specific software developed for the present study will be used to depict dot-plot figures. We will address possible publication bias using two complementary statistics, the funnel plot and the Egger’s regression test. The trim-and-fill method [59, 60] will reveal the number of effect sizes necessary to be imputed to restore the symmetry of the funnel plot.

The fact that this review includes only articles written in English may be a limitation. However, clearly, most studies are reported in English, and it is expected to extract very comprehensive information.

The central research question of this study is whether all training components recommended by official bodies are efficient for enhancing EFs and episodic memory and whether moderators, such as exercise program types and participants’ characteristics, could influence the effect size of the effect of regular exercise on cognitive aging [46, 61].

This systematic review and multilevel meta-analysis will provide evidence on how to optimize physical activity programs to improve and/or maintain these cognitive functions that decline more with age. So, the results of the present study would contribute to identify the gaps and limitations of current physical exercise research on executive functions and episodic memory in older adults. It would also allow to understand the quality of the research conducted to date in this field and summarize its main findings. The findings of this study will be useful for clinicians, physical trainer specialists, psychologists, social workers, and gerontologists, as well as older adults, their families, and wider public.

Ethics and dissemination

This systematic review and meta-analytic study do not require approval from an ethics committee. The results will be disseminated in peer-reviewed journals and at international conferences and scientific meetings.

Abbreviations

CIs:

Confidence intervals

EFs:

Executive functions

EM:

Episodic memory

FITT:

Frequency, intensity, type, and time of exercise

MD:

Mean difference

PA:

Physical activity

RoB:

Risk of bias

SMD:

Standardized mean difference

WM:

Working memory

References

  1. United Nations Department of Economic and Social Affairs PD. World Population Prospects 2022: Summary of Results. UN DESA/POP/2022/TR/NO. 3. University Press; 2022.

  2. European Commission. The 2018 Ageing Report: economic and budgetary projections for the EU Member States. 2018.

    Google Scholar 

  3. Hou Y, Dan X, Babbar M, Wei Y, Hasselbalch SG, Croteau DL, et al. Ageing as a risk factor for neurodegenerative disease. Nat Rev Neurol. 2019;15(10):565–81.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Ballesteros S, Kraft E, Santana S, Tziraki C. Maintaining older brain functionality: a targeted review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2015;55:453–77.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Ballesteros S, Voelcker-Rehage C, Bherer L. Editorial: cognitive and brain plasticity induced by physical exercise, cognitive training, video games, and combined interventions. Front Hum Neurosci. 2018;7:12.

    Google Scholar 

  6. Colcombe S, Kramer AF. Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults: a meta-analytic study. Psychol Sci. 2003;14(2):125–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Kramer AF, Hahn S, Cohen NJ, Banich MT, McAuley E, Harrison CR, et al. Ageing, fitness and neurocognitive function. Nature. 1999;400(6743):418–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Audiffren M, André N. Exercise and aging. In: Sport, Exercise and Performance Psychology. UK: Oxford University Press; 2021. p. 238–49.

  9. Bherer L, Erickson KI, Liu-Ambrose T. A review of the effects of physical activity and exercise on cognitive and brain functions in older adults. J Aging Res. 2013;2013:657508.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Voelcker-Rehage C, Niemann C. Structural and functional brain changes related to different types of physical activity across the life span. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2013;37(9):2268–95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Bangsbo J, Blackwell J, Boraxbekk CJ, Caserotti P, Dela F, Evans AB, et al. Copenhagen Consensus statement 2019: physical activity and ageing. Br J Sports Med. 2019;53(14):856–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Hoyer WJ, Verhaeghen P. Memory aging. In: Handbook of the Psychology of Aging. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 2006. p. 209–32.

  13. Cabeza R, Nyberg L, Park DC. Cognitive neuroscience of aging: lining cognition and cerebral aging. UK: Oxford University Press; 2005.

  14. Park DC, Lautenschlager G, Hedden T, Davidson NS, Smith AD, Smith PK. Models of visuospatial and verbal memory across the adult life span. Psychol Aging. 2002;17(2):299–320.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Kachouri H, Fay S, Angel L, Isingrini M. Influence of current physical exercise on the relationship between aging and episodic memory and fluid intelligence. Acta Psychol (Amst). 2022;227:103609.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Liu-Ambrose T, Nagamatsu LS, Graf P, Beattie BL, Ashe MC, Handy TC. Resistance training and executive functions: a 12-month randomized controlled trial. Arch Intern Med. 2010;170(2):170–8. Available from:  https://biblioproxy.uqtr.ca/login?url=https://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=mnh&AN=20101012&site=ehost-live.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Moutoussamy I, Taconnat L, Pothier K, Toussaint L, Fay S. Episodic memory and aging: benefits of physical activity depend on the executive resources required for the task. PLoS ONE. 2022;17(2):e0263919.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Nouchi R, Taki Y, Takeuchi H, Sekiguchi A, Hashizume H, Nozawa T, et al. Four weeks of combination exercise training improved executive functions, episodic memory, and processing speed in healthy elderly people: evidence from a randomized controlled trial. Age (Dordr). 2014;36(2):787–99. Available from: https://biblioproxy.uqtr.ca/login?url=https://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=mnh&AN=24065294&site=ehost-live.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Diamond A. Executive functions. Annu Rev Psychol. 2013;64(1):135–68.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Friedman NP, Miyake A. Unity and diversity of executive functions: Individual differences as a window on cognitive structure. Cortex. 2017;86:186–204.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Panikratova YR, Vlasova RM, Akhutina TV, Korneev AA, Sinitsyn VE, Pechenkova EV. Functional connectivity of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex contributes to different components of executive functions. Int J Psychophysiol. 2020;151:70–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Hasher L, Zacks RT. Working memory, comprehension, and aging: a review and a new view. 1988. p. 193–225.

    Google Scholar 

  23. Gazzaley A, Cooney JW, Rissman J, D’Esposito M. Top-down suppression deficit underlies working memory impairment in normal aging. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8(10):1298–300.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Idowu MI, Szameitat AJ. Executive function abilities in cognitively healthy young and older adults—a cross-sectional study. Front Aging Neurosci. 2023;8:15.

    Google Scholar 

  25. Baddeley AD, Hitch GJ. Developments in the concept of working memory. Neuropsychology. 1994;8(4):485–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Salthouse TA. Influence of processing speed on adult age differences in working memory. Acta Psychol (Amst). 1992;79(2):155–70.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Zimprich D, Kurtz T. Individual differences and predictors of forgetting in old age: the role of processing speed and working memory. Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 2013;20(2):195–219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Ionescu T. Exploring the nature of cognitive flexibility. New Ideas Psychol. 2012;30(2):190–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Tulving E. Elements of episodic memory. New York: Oxford University Press; 1983.

    Google Scholar 

  30. Tulving E. Episodic memory: from mind to brain. Annu Rev Psychol. 2002;53(1):1–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Rhodes S, Greene NR, Naveh-Benjamin M. Age-related differences in recall and recognition: a meta-analysis. Psychon Bull Rev. 2019;26(5):1529–47.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Rugg MD, Vilberg KL. Brain networks underlying episodic memory retrieval. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2013;23(2):255–60.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Aghjayan SL, Bournias T, Kang C, Zhou X, Stillman CM, Donofry SD, et al. Aerobic exercise improves episodic memory in late adulthood: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Commun Med. 2022;2:15.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Balbim GM, Falck RS, Barha CK, Starkey SY, Bullock A, Davis JC, et al. Effects of exercise training on the cognitive function of older adults with different types of dementia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Br J Sports Med. 2022;56(16):933–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Biazus-Sehn LF, Schuch FB, Firth J, de Stigger F S. Effects of physical exercise on cognitive function of older adults with mild cognitive impairment: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2020;89:104048.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Chen FT, Etnier JL, Chan KH, Chiu PK, Hung TM, Chang YK. Effects of exercise training interventions on executive function in older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sports Med. 2020;50(8):1451–67.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Coelho-Junior H, Marzetti E, Calvani R, Picca A, Arai H, Uchida M. Resistance training improves cognitive function in older adults with different cognitive status: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Aging Ment Health. 2022;26(2):213–24.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Da Silva JL, Agbangla NF, Le Page C, Ghernout W, Andrieu B. Effects of chronic physical exercise or multicomponent exercise programs on the mental health and cognition of older adults living in a nursing home: a systematic review of studies from the past 10 years. Front Psychol. 2022;13:13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Falck RS, Davis JC, Best JR, Crockett RA, Liu-Ambrose T. Impact of exercise training on physical and cognitive function among older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurobiol Aging. 2019;79:119–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Gallardo-Gómez D, del Pozo-Cruz J, Noetel M, Álvarez-Barbosa F, Alfonso-Rosa RM, del Pozo CB. Optimal dose and type of exercise to improve cognitive function in older adults: a systematic review and Bayesian model-based network meta-analysis of RCTs. Ageing Res Rev. 2022;76:101591.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Huang X, Zhao X, Li B, Cai Y, Zhang S, Wan Q, et al. Comparative efficacy of various exercise interventions on cognitive function in patients with mild cognitive impairment or dementia: a systematic review and network meta-analysis. J Sport Health Sci. 2022;11(2):212–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Jiang J, Guo W, Wang B. Effects of exergaming on executive function of older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. PeerJ. 2022;11(10):e13194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Lin M, Ma C, Zhu J, Gao J, Huang L, Huang J, et al. Effects of exercise interventions on executive function in old adults with mild cognitive impairment: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Ageing Res Rev. 2022;82:101776.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Liu X, Wang G, Cao Y. Association of nonpharmacological interventions for cognitive function in older adults with mild cognitive impairment: a systematic review and network meta-analysis. Aging Clin Exp Res. 2023;35(3):463–78.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Northey JM, Cherbuin N, Pumpa KL, Smee DJ, Rattray B. Exercise interventions for cognitive function in adults older than 50: a systematic review with meta-analysis. Br J Sports Med. 2018;52(3):154–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Rieker JA, Reales JM, Muiños M, Ballesteros S. The effects of combined cognitive-physical interventions on cognitive functioning in healthy older adults: a systematic review and multilevel meta-analysis. Front Hum Neurosci. 2022;24:16.

    Google Scholar 

  47. Sanders LMJ, Hortobágyi T, la Bastide-van GS, van der Zee EA. van Heuvelen MJG Dose-response relationship between exercise and cognitive function in older adults with and without cognitive impairment: a systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS ONE. 2019;14(1):e0210036. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210036.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Venegas-Sanabria LC, Cavero-Redondo I, Martínez-Vizcaino V, Cano-Gutierrez CA, Álvarez-Bueno C. Effect of multicomponent exercise in cognitive impairment: a systematic review and meta-analysis. BMC Geriatr. 2022;22(1):617.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  49. Brach M, de Bruin ED, Levin O, Hinrichs T, Zijlstra W, Netz Y. Evidence-based yet still challenging! Research on physical activity in old age. Eur Rev Aging Phys Act. 2023;20(1):7.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Higgins JPT, Green S. Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions 4.2.6. Vol. 4. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd; 2006.

    Google Scholar 

  51. Moher D, Shamseer L, Clarke M, Ghersi D, Liberati A, Petticrew M, et al. Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis Protocols (PRISMA-P) 2015 statement. Syst Rev. 2015;4(1):1.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  52. Ouzzani M, Hammady H, Fedorowicz Z, Elmagarmid A. Rayyan—a web and mobile app for systematic reviews. Syst Rev. 2016;5(1):210.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  53. Sterne JAC, Savović J, Page MJ, Elbers RG, Blencowe NS, Boutron I, et al. RoB 2: a revised tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials. BMJ. 2019;28:l4898.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Whiting P, Savović J, Higgins JPT, Caldwell DM, Reeves BC, Shea B, et al. ROBIS: a new tool to assess risk of bias in systematic reviews was developed. J Clin Epidemiol. 2016;69:225–34.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Viechtbauer W. Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. J Stat Softw. 2010;36(3):1–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. R Core Team. A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2021. http://www.R-project.org

  57. Assink M, Wibbelink CJM. Fitting three-level meta-analytic models in R: a step-by-step tutorial. Quant Method Psychol. 2016;12(3):154–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Egger M, Davey Smith G, Schneider M, Minder C. Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ. 1997;315(7109):629–34.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  59. Duval S, Tweedie R. Trim and fill: a simple funnel-plot–based method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Biometrics. 2000;56(2):455–63.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Duval S, Tweedie R. A nonparametric “trim and fill” method of accounting for publication bias in meta-analysis. J Am Stat Assoc. 2000;95(449):89–98.

    Google Scholar 

  61. Netz Y. Is there a preferred mode of exercise for cognition enhancement in older age? A narrative review. Front Med (Lausanne). 2019;29:6.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

This article is the result of work from COST Action CA20104 — network on evidence-based physical activity in old age (PhysAgeNet), supported by COST (European Cooperation in Science and Technology). https://www.cost.eu, https://physagenet.eu/. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

SB, YN, MA, and CV-R conceptualized the design. SB wrote the original draft, and YN, MA, and CV-R reviewed and edited the draft. Funding acquisition, supervision, validation, and visualization were done by YN and SB. JMR and MA dealt with the methodology issues. All authors have taken part in initiating the idea, deciding the exercise types, the search terms, the specific measures of the outcomes, and the inclusion/exclusion criteria. All authors read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Soledad Ballesteros.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Not applicable.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Ballesteros, S., Audifren, M., Badache, A. et al. Effects of chronic physical exercise on executive functions and episodic memory in clinical and healthy older adult populations: a systematic review and meta-analysis protocol. Syst Rev 13, 98 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13643-024-02517-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13643-024-02517-0

Keywords