Skip to main content

Do descriptive norms messaging interventions backfire? Protocol for a systematic review of the boomerang effect

Abstract

Background

Descriptive norms messaging interventions are used to motivate people to adopt or maintain desirable behaviors. Such interventions provide people with information that describes an undesirable behavior as uncommon or a desirable behavior as prevalent within a relevant social group. Descriptive norms messaging interventions have shown promise in increasing individual and social benefit for a broad range of health and sustainability programs. However, evidence suggests that people who have adopted desirable behaviors sometimes regress to undesirable behaviors after receiving descriptive norms messages due to the type of information provided in the messages. This phenomenon is called the boomerang effect. We aim to conduct a systematic review of boomerang effects on health and environmental sustainability behaviors resulting from exposure to descriptive norms messaging interventions.

Methods

We will employ our search strategy to identify studies of descriptive norms messaging interventions published prior to December 31, 2020. We will search the Cochrane Library, Campbell Library, PsycINFO, PubMed, Social Science Research Network (SSRN), and Web of Science to retrieve peer-reviewed articles published in English. We will restrict inclusion to studies (e.g., randomized and non-randomized controlled trials, quasi-experimental studies, and observational studies) of health and environmental sustainability interventions that assess behaviors before and after exposure to descriptive norms messaging. Two reviewers will independently extract data about study populations and design, intervention components, and behavioral measures. We will use the revised Cochrane Risk of Bias assessment tool (RoB2) and Risk Of Bias in Non-randomized Studies—of Intervention (ROBINS-I) to assess the risk of bias, and the Liverpool Quality Assessment Tool (LQAT) to assess the quality of evidence. We will conduct thematic analyses to codify interventions, and examine intervention effects across subgroups of individuals based on their behavior prior to intervention exposure (e.g., those practicing desirable behaviors vs. undesirable behaviors). We will also conduct moderator analyses to determine whether boomerang effects are contingent upon other factors including intervention framing and delivery modality.

Discussion

This systematic review will provide information about descriptive norms messaging intervention effects across subgroups of individuals and elucidate factors that potentially moderate boomerang effects. The review will yield evidence-based recommendations for the structure and content of descriptive norms messages that can be employed to avoid unintended boomerang effects within the context of health and sustainability programming.

Systematic review registration

PROSPERO CRD42020156989

Peer Review reports

Background

Humans are social beings; they seek information about what relevant others do and adjust their behavior to fit in to the group [1, 2]. When people believe that a crowd behaves in a certain way, they are likely to conform to what the crowd does [3]. These rules that govern behavioral expectations within groups are called social norms [4]. A behavioral pattern that is conditional only on a person’s belief about what is commonly done is known as a descriptive norm [5].

The power of conformity has inspired a type of behavior change technique called descriptive norms messaging. Interventions that employ descriptive norms messaging aim to motivate people to adopt or maintain desirable behaviors by providing people with information that describes an undesirable behavior as uncommon or a desirable behavior as prevalent within a relevant social group [3].

Program implementers and researchers have utilized descriptive norms messaging as an intervention technique to increase individual and social benefit for a broad range of public health and sustainability programming—from increasing vegetable consumption [6] and discouraging alcohol misuse amongst college students [7] to reducing plastic bag use [8]. However, evidence suggests that people who have adopted desirable behaviors sometimes regress to undesirable behaviors after receiving descriptive norms messages due to the type of information provided in the messaging [9, 10]. This phenomenon is called the boomerang effect [10]. The boomerang effect refers to when people who exhibit a desirable behavior become less likely to do so after being exposed to descriptive information about others that indicates the desirable behavior is less frequent, occurs to a lesser degree, or is not exhibited amongst other people in relevant social groups [10]. Studies examining boomerang effects after exposure to descriptive norms messaging interventions have yielded inconsistent results. One study, for example, found that households with relatively low energy use increased their energy consumption after receiving descriptive information about their neighbors’ average energy use, as the average was higher than their own relatively low consumption levels [10]. Conversely, a study of an intervention designed to correct college students’ exaggerated perceptions of alcohol use on college campuses found that lighter drinkers who drink less than the typical student did not increase drinking after receiving personalized descriptive norms messaging [11]. This inconclusive evidence regarding the intervention effects of descriptive norms interventions warrants further examination into potential, yet unintended boomerang effects that result from exposure to descriptive information regarding relevant others’ behaviors.

Previous systematic reviews have examined the effectiveness of descriptive norms messaging interventions on targeted behaviors amongst entire intervention populations [12,13,14] and the unintended boomerang effects of information-based interventions [15, 16]. However, to our knowledge, no known reviews have examined boomerang and differential effects of descriptive norms messaging interventions amongst subgroups of intervention populations based on their behavior prior to intervention exposure. To fill in this gap, our systematic review will synthesize evidence regarding potential boomerang effects of descriptive norms messaging interventions designed to change behaviors related to health and environmental sustainability. More specifically, the aim of this review is threefold. First, we aim to examine peer-reviewed literature evaluating descriptive norms messaging interventions that promote health and environmental sustainability behaviors. Second, we aim to synthesize evidence regarding intervention effects on subgroups that behave differently prior to intervention exposure, and determine the magnitude and direction of such effects on people who practice desirable behaviors prior to being exposed to descriptive information that indicates others practice the desirable behavior to a lesser extent or not at all (i.e., boomerang effect). Third, we aim to identify factors that account for heterogeneity of boomerang effects.

Methods/design

This protocol has been registered within the PROSPERO database (registration number CRD42020156989) and is being reported in accordance with the reporting guidance provided in the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses Protocols (PRISMA-P) statement [17, 18] (see checklist in Additional file 1).

Eligibility criteria

Eligibility criteria for this review align with the PICO-T framework, which defines populations, interventions, comparators, outcomes of interest, and types of studies eligible for review [19].

Types of studies

To allow for examinations of behavior pre- and post-intervention exposure, we will restrict our review to ex-ante randomized and non-randomized controlled trials, controlled before-and-after studies, interrupted-time-series studies, uncontrolled before-and-after studies, and case series (uncontrolled longitudinal studies).

Types of populations

We will review studies involving healthy populations, including children, adolescents, and adult population (regardless of age or sex).

Types of interventions

Interventions of interest for this systematic review include health and environmental sustainability interventions that employ descriptive norms messaging, where exposed groups receive summary information regarding others’ behaviors (e.g., proportion of others engaged in the desirable behavior, total number of people engaged in the behavior, mean or median number of people who have adopted the desirable behavior within a relevant social group). We will not limit inclusion based on the duration, frequency, or method of intervention exposure.

Types of comparators

We will include studies that make comparisons between groups receiving descriptive norms messaging interventions and counterfactual comparator groups (e.g., groups received alternative interventions or no interventions).

Types of outcome measures

The primary outcomes of interest are changes in behavior related to health and environmental sustainability after exposure to descriptive norms messaging interventions. Specifically, the measures of behavior change may include changes in the prevalence of the desirable behavior (e.g., proportion of people engaging in the behavior) or the degree or frequency of performance of the desirable behavior (e.g., the average degree or frequency of the behavior). As such, we will restrict studies to those that captured both pre-intervention and post-intervention measures of the desirable behaviors.

Exclusion criteria

We will exclude studies if the description of study methods is incomplete or ambiguous to the extent that renders reviewers unable to determine whether the study meets all inclusion criteria. If authors fail to report sufficient statistics or data for estimating changes in behavior before and after intervention exposure, we will include the related studies in the review and thematic synthesis, but not the meta-analyses [20]. Exclusion criteria are subject to change during screening and selection. If changes are made, they will be reported in the final review paper and applied retroactively to all previously screened studies.

Information sources and search strategy

We will harvest data from literature written in English and published in peer-reviewed journals prior to December 31, 2020. We will use search terms and Boolean operators to identify articles that may be eligible for review. We will systematically search the following electronic databases: Cochrane Library, Campbell Library, PsycINFO, PubMed, Social Science Research Network (SSRN), and Web of Science. For each database, we will first employ keyword searches (e.g., descriptive norms, health behaviors), then apply MeSH terms (if applicable), text words, and phrases associated with the keywords. We will then generate preliminary search results by combining all keyword search sets with appropriate Boolean operators. Literature generated from the searches will be exported into a reference management software (EndNote). Duplicates will be removed prior to screening. Our detailed search strategy for MEDLINE is described in Additional file 2. As various electronic databases have their own respective search platforms that operationalize search strategies, we will modify our search procedures, as needed based on the respective databases. We will report the full database search process, including all iterations used to search all targeted databases when our searches are complete.

Study screening and selection

We will perform screening and selection in a stepwise manner. First, two reviewers will independently screen the titles and abstracts of papers identified during the database search, using our pre-established inclusion and exclusion criteria to identify eligible studies. The reviewers will exclude studies if their titles and abstracts (a) meet any exclusion criteria or (b) clearly fail to meet all inclusion criteria. If the reviewers are not able to make a definitive decision based on the title and abstract, they will independently review the full text of the paper to determine eligibility. The two reviewers will independently review the full text of all papers that are determined eligible upon title and abstract review such that studies included in the review meet all inclusion and no exclusion criteria.

If disagreements arise between reviewers during the study screening and selection process, they will be resolved by discussion with a third reviewer. If disagreements arise due to a lack of information, we will contact the primary study authors for clarification. We will record and report disagreements and their resolutions. We will use Cohen’s kappa to assess inter-rater reliability between reviewers [21].

Data extraction and coding

Two reviewers will independently extract relevant data from all studies included in the review using a standardized data extraction form (see Additional file 3). We will pilot test the data extraction form using two randomly selected articles, and refine it, as needed. If relevant data are unclear or unreported, we will contact the authors for clarification. We will harvest data about study populations (including age and gender distributions) and settings (including country and region), details related to study design and interventions (including intervention details such as descriptive norms message design and any non-descriptive norms co-interventions, and control details, if any), and statistical methods. We will extract data on behavioral measures both pre- and post-intervention exposure. For dichotomous outcomes, we will extract the number of participants who practice desirable and undesirable behaviors and ratio measures with standard errors, if available. For count outcomes, we will extract the number of episodes the desirable or undesirable behaviors were practiced (e.g., frequency of excessive alcohol consumption per week across study arms with the total person-time in each study arm the rate ratio) and standard errors, if available.

We will extract statistical information from included studies based on the type of data collected, the type of statistical significance tests performed, the effect size measures used, and the types of statistics information reported. If the study specifically reports data related to individuals who outperform the descriptive norms messaging information in the pre-treatment measure, we will extract the pre- and post-treatment sample size. If the study reports the effect size d, it will be entered; if the study does not report effect size data for the groups of interest, but the authors report sufficient information, we will calculate the estimated effect size estimator d. If the study meets inclusion criteria but does not report sufficient data to calculate the effect size, we will contact the corresponding author to request the information or dataset, and perform effect size calculation ourselves. We will code effect sizes as positive if the performance of the behavior increased in the desired direction after intervention exposure. In contrast, we will code effect sizes as negative if the performance of the behavior does not increase in the desired direction after intervention exposure. If the authors indicate that the effect was simply “non-significant,” we will enter a zero effect size and p value of 0.50 [22, 23].

We are aware that the level of intervention and analysis will likely vary across studies. Some studies likely focused on individual behavioral change or maintenance while others likely addressed household-level behavioral change or maintenance. As a result, we will calculate and present effect estimates based on the level at which the descriptive norms interventions were administered (e.g., individual, household, community).

Risk of bias assessments

Two reviewers will independently assess the risk of bias for each included study using the revised Cochrane Risk of Bias assessment tool (RoB2) for randomized trials and Risk Of Bias in Non-randomized Studies—of Intervention (ROBINS-I) for non-randomized studies of interventions [24, 25]. The assessments will be conducted at the study level, following the guidance accordingly. To eliminate the possibility of bias in assessing quality, author names and affiliations may be removed from reports before they are evaluated. We will report the assessment results and conduct a sensitivity analysis to estimate the potential impact of studies at high risk of bias on the overall conclusion.

Quality of evidence appraisal

We will use the Liverpool Quality Assessment Tool (LQAT) to critically assess the quality of evidence included in the review [26]. We will use the following criteria to assess the quality of evidence presented in the review: (a) selection procedures, (b) baseline assessment, (c) outcome assessment, (d) analysis/confounding, and (e) contribution of evidence towards review questions that are rated as strong, moderate or weak.

Data synthesis

We will conduct our analyses in two phases, which we summarize here and detail further in subsequent sub-sections. First, we will conduct qualitative analyses to codify intervention themes, examine descriptive norms messaging structure and content, and produce a narrative synthesis of studies included in the review. Then, we will examine overall trends in behaviors before and after intervention exposure, and the heterogeneity of results presented in included studies. We will carry out meta-analyses if it is appropriate to do so based on the heterogeneity of studies, targeted behaviors and levels of intervention, and behavioral measures. We will also assess publication bias and perform moderator analyses. We will perform our meta-analysis in RStudio (version 1.1.463) using the “metaphor” library to do so [27]. Our pre-defined type I error threshold is alpha = 0.05 on two-tailed tests.

Qualitative analyses

Given the scope of this systematic review and the descriptive nature of the intervention messages (i.e., providing statistical information of others’ behaviors), we anticipate heterogeneity amongst the included studies. Regardless of whether we are able to perform meta-analyses, we will tabulate the characteristics of the included studies and conduct a thematic analysis following the procedure suggested by Thomas and Harden [28]. During our qualitative analysis phase, we will code the intervention messages, organize them into descriptive themes, use those themes to investigate the types of intervention messages that are effective at changing behavior in the desirable direction, and identify factors that may moderate the results (e.g., targeted populations, behaviors of interest). We will report results by intervention domain (e.g., energy conservation, vegetable consumption, alcohol consumption) and separately for subgroups based on whether individuals practiced desirable behaviors prior to intervention exposure.

Meta-analyses

Given the scope of our research questions and the anticipated heterogeneity of studies emerging from various domains, we assume studies included in the review will not estimate the same underlying population parameters. Therefore, we will use random effects models and employ the restricted maximum likelihood estimation method [29]⁠.

To compute the weighted mean of the effect sizes, we will assign weight to certain studies based on the inverse of the associated fixed effects and/or random effects variances [30]. This method has been shown to outperform weighting by sample size in random effects analyses [31].

To test for heterogeneity of the effect sizes, we will use the homogeneity statistic (Q), which has a chi-square distribution with degrees of freedom equal to the total number of effect sizes minus one (e – 1) [27, 28]. We will also use the I2 statistic as a second measure of heterogeneity, which is more useful to compare across meta-analyses and is less dependent on the number of synthesized effects [30, 32]⁠.

Outlying effect sizes are defined as effect sizes that are three standard deviations larger or smaller than the mean. Following the winsorization method suggested by Dixon and Tukey, we will minimize any outlying effects and replace them with the next most extreme values [33, 34].

Publication bias

We will address publication bias through the use of the following three strategies. First, we will address the “file drawer problem” by computing the number of fail-safe on individual effect sizes with Rosenberg’s calculator [35]⁠. Second, we will present funnel plots, with the y-axes representing study precision (i.e., error of the intervention effect estimates in a reverse scale) and the x-axes representing intervention effect size estimates (i.e., standardized mean difference), as suggested by Sterne and Egger [36]⁠. To statistically test and adjust for publication bias with funnel plots, we will use the trim-and-fill method [37]. Third, we will examine the Hedges’ d values of individual effect sizes in a normal-quantile plot. If the effect sizes are from a normal distribution, the data point (effect size of individual studies) will rest near the diagonal of X Y; deviation will suggest publication bias [38].

Moderator analysis

We will perform moderator analyses to examine whether the variance of study-specific treatment effects is systematically associated with aspects of study designs and/or population characteristics. Proposed moderators include the nature and types of summary information provided in the descriptive norms messages (e.g., information about behavior summarized as proportions, total numbers, mean, or median of the reference population), delivery modality (e.g., personalized feedback, public messaging), and population characteristics (e.g., age and gender distributions, if any). This list of moderators will be further refined based on findings from our thematic syntheses. We will perform a power determination prior to the moderator analyses to aid in the interpretation of the results [39].

Discussion

This systematic review and the related thematic and meta-analyses will contribute evidence that may enhance understanding regarding the effect of descriptive norms interventions on subgroups with different behavioral patterns prior to intervention exposure. Findings from this study can provide information to practitioners and policy makers who plan to leverage descriptive norms messaging to encourage the adoption and maintenance of improved health and environmental sustainability behaviors. While descriptive norms messaging is common in these applied settings, there is a lack of systematically derived advice on when practitioners or researchers who are designing these interventions should account for possible boomerang effects, and how they can do so most effectively. In particular, this study will provide evidence regarding specific intervention messaging characteristics that are effective and the factors that need to be taken into consideration when designing, planning, and executing such interventions. It is worth emphasizing here that this study will synthesize and integrate empirical evidence across study domains and will provide insights on multidisciplinary collaborations. Complex and interconnected problems such as those arising in the environmental and public health arenas will likely require the examination of rigorous evidence emerging across disciplines to identify effective, interdisciplinary solutions.

Potential limitations of this review include (1) the consideration of studies published only in the English language for inclusion, (2) the restriction of literature searches to the selected databases, (3) the exclusion of studies that did not report the pre-intervention behavior measures. Results will be disseminated through publication in a peer-reviewed journal. Any amendments made to this protocol during the review will be outlined in PROSPERO and reported in the final manuscript.

Availability of data and materials

Not applicable

Abbreviations

PRISMA-P:

Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Review and Meta-analysis Protocols

PROSPERO:

International Prospective Register of Systematic Reviews

RCTs:

Randomized controlled trials

RoB2:

Revised Cochrane Risk of Bias assessment tool

ROBINS-I:

Risk Of Bias in Non-randomized Studies—of Intervention

LQAT:

Liverpool Quality Assessment Tool

References

  1. Cialdini RB, Goldstein NJ. Social influence: compliance and conformity. Annual Review of Psychology [Internet]. Annual Reviews; 2004 Feb;55(1):591–621. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.142015.

  2. Bicchieri C, Xiao E. Do the right thing: but only if others do so. J Behav Decis Mak [Internet]. 2009 1 [cited 2019 Jun 15];22(2):191–208. Available from: http://doi.wiley.com/10.1002/bdm.621.

  3. Rimal RN, Real K. Understanding the influence of perceived norms on behaviors. Commun Theory [Internet]. 2003 May 1 [cited 2019 Jun 17];13(2):184–203. Available from: https://academic.oup.com/ct/article/13/2/184-203/4110437.

  4. Bicchieri C. The grammar of society: Cambridge University Press; 2005. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1017/cbo9780511616037.

  5. Bicchieri C, Xiao E. Do the right thing: but only if others do so. J Behav Decis Mak [Internet]. 2009 1 [cited 2019 Feb 1];22(2):191–208. Available from: http://doi.wiley.com/10.1002/bdm.621.

  6. Thomas JM, Robinson EL, Aveyard P, Jebb SA, Herman CP, Higgs S. Using a descriptive norm message to increase vegetable selection in a workplace restaurant setting. Appetite [Internet]. Elsevier BV. 2016;107:692–3 Available from: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2016.08.090.

    Google Scholar 

  7. Neighbors C, DiBello AM, Young CM, Steers MN, Rinker DV, Rodriguez LM, Ryamond Knee C, Blanton H, Lewis MA. Personalized normative feedback for heavy drinking: an application of deviance regulation theory. Behav Res Ther. 2019 Apr;115:73-82. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2018.11.004. Epub 2018 Nov 13. PMID: 30580836; PMCID: PMC6759083.

  8. De Groot J, Abrahamse W, Jones K. Persuasive normative messages: the influence of injunctive and personal norms on using free plastic bags. Sustainability [Internet]. MDPI AG; 2013 Apr 29;5(5):1829–44. Available from: https://doi.org/10.3390/su5051829.

  9. Verkooijen KTKT, Stok FMM, Mollen S. The power of regression to the mean: a social norm study revisited. Eur J Soc Psychol. 2015 Jun;45(4):417–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Schultz PW, Nolan JM, Cialdini RB, Goldstein NJ, Griskevicius V. The constructive, destructive and reconstructive power of social norms. Psychol Sci. 2007 May;18(5):429–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Prince MA, Reid A, Carey KB, Neighbors C. Effects of normative feedback for drinkers who consume less than the norm: dodging the boomerang. Psychol Addict Behav. 2014;28(2):538–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Borsari B, Carey KB. Descriptive and injunctive norms in college drinking: a meta-analytic integration. J Stud Alcohol. 2003 May;64(3):331–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Lewis MA, Neighbors C. Social norms approaches using descriptive drinking norms education: a review of the research on personalized normative feedback. J Am Coll Heal. 2006;54(4):213–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Ringold DJ. Boomerang effects in response to public health interventions: some unintended consequences in the alcoholic beverage market [Internet]. Vol. 25, Journal of Consumer Policy. Journal of Consumer Policy; 2002 [cited 2018 Nov 6]. p. 27–63. Available from: https://insights.ovid.com/jcop/200225010/00040679-200225010-00002.

  15. Byrne S, Hart PS. The boomerang effect a synthesis of findings and a preliminary theoretical framework. Ann Int Commun Assoc [Internet]. 2009 [cited 2019 Dec 8];33(1):3–37. Available from: https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=rica20.

  16. Cho H, Salmon CT. Unintended effects of health communication campaigns. J Commun [Internet]. 2007 Jun 1 [cited 2019 Dec 8];57(2):293–317. Available from: https://academic.oup.com/joc/article/57/2/293-317/4102644.

  17. Moher D, Shamseer L, Clarke M, Ghersi D, Liberati A, Petticrew M, et al. Preferred reporting items for systematic review and meta-analysis protocols (PRISMA-P) 2015 statement. Syst Rev. 2015;4(1). https://doi.org/10.1186/2046-4053-4-1.

  18. Shamseer L, Moher D, Clarke M, Ghersi D, Liberati A, Petticrew M, et al. Preferred reporting items for systematic review and meta-analysis protocols (PRISMA-P) 2015: elaboration and explanation. BMJ. 2015;350:g7647 https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.g7647.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Richardson WS, Wilson MC, Nishikawa J, Hayward RS. The well-built clinical question: a key to evidence-based decisions. ACP J Club. 1995;123(3):A12–3. 7582737.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Meline T. Selecting studies for systematic review: inclusion and exclusion criteria. Contemp Issues Commun Sci Disord [Internet]. 2006 [cited 2019 3];33:21–7. Available from: https://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/a6b4/d6d01bd19a67e794db4b70207a45d47d82f3.pdf.

  21. McHugh ML. Interrater reliability: the kappa statistic. Biochem Medica. 2012;22(3):276–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Rosenthal NE, Sack DA, Gillin JC, Lewy AJ, Goodwin FK, Davenport Y, et al. Seasonal affective disorder. A description of the syndrome and preliminary findings with light therapy. Arch Gen Psychiatry [Internet]. 1984 Jan [cited 2019 Feb 7];41(1):72–80. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/6581756.

  23. Mullen B, Driskell JE, Salas E. Meta-analysis and the study of group dynamics. Group dynamics: theory, research, and practice [Internet]. American Psychological Association (APA); 1998;2(4):213–29. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2699.2.4.213.

  24. Sterne JAC, Savović J, Page MJ, Elbers RG, Blencowe NS, Boutron I, et al. RoB 2: a revised tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials. BMJ [Internet]. BMJ; 2019 28;l4898. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.l4898.

  25. Sterne JA, Hernán MA, Reeves BC, Savović J, Berkman ND, Viswanathan M, et al. ROBINS-I: a tool for assessing risk of bias in non-randomised studies of interventions. BMJ [Internet]. BMJ; 2016 Oct 12;i4919. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.i4919.

  26. Pope D, Bruce N. IG and RE. Methodological quality of individual studies was assessed using Liverpool Quality Assessment Tools. University of Liverpool. .

  27. Viechtbauer W. Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. J Stat Softw [Internet]. 2010 5 [cited 2019 Feb 7];36(3):1–48. Available from: http://www.jstatsoft.org/v36/i03/.

  28. Thomas J, Harden A. Methods for the thematic synthesis of qualitative research in systematic reviews. BMC Med Res Methodol [Internet]. 2008 Dec 10 [cited 2019 Jun 20];8(1):45. Available from: https://bmcmedresmethodol.biomedcentral.com/articles/10.1186/1471-2288-8-45.

  29. Marín-Martínez F, Sánchez-Meca J. Weighting by inverse variance or by sample size in random-effects meta-analysis. Educ Psychol Meas [Internet]. 2010 Feb [cited 2019 Feb 7];70(1):56–73. Available from: http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/0013164409344534.

  30. Borenstein M. Hedges LV. Rothstein HR. Introduction to meta-analysis. John Wiley & Sons, Ltd: Higgins JPT; 2009. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470743386.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Hedges L V., Vevea JL. Fixed- and random-effects models in meta-analysis. Psychol Methods [Internet]. 1998 [cited 2019 Feb 7];3(4):486–504. Available from: http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi = 10.1037/1082-989X.3.4.486.

  32. Higgins JP, Green S, editors. Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions [Internet]. Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd; 2008 [cited 2019 Jun 17]. Available from: http://doi.wiley.com/10.1002/9780470712184.

  33. Tukey JW. The future of data analysis. Ann Math Stat [Internet]. 1962 Mar [cited 2019 Jun 20];33(1):1–67. Available from: http://projecteuclid.org/euclid.aoms/1177704711.

  34. Dixon WJ. Simplified estimation from censored normal samples. Ann Math Stat [Internet]. 1960 Jun [cited 2019 Jun 20];31(2):385–91. Available from: http://projecteuclid.org/euclid.aoms/1177705900.

  35. Rosenberg MS. The file-drawer problem revisited: a general weighted method for calculating fail-safe numbers in meta-analysis. Evolution [Internet]. 2005 [cited 2019 Jun 20];59(2):464–8. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15807430.

  36. Sterne JA, Egger M. Funnel plots for detecting bias in meta-analysis: guidelines on choice of axis. J Clin Epidemiol [Internet]. 2001 [cited 2019 Jun 20];54(10):1046–55. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11576817.

  37. Duval S, Tweedie R. A nonparametric “trim and fill” method of accounting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Journal of the American Statistical Association [Internet]. Informa UK Limited; 2000 Mar;95(449):89–98. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1080/01621459.2000.10473905.

  38. Wang MC, Bushman BJ. Using the normal quantile plot to explore meta-analytic data sets. Psychological Methods [Internet]. American Psychological Association (APA); 1998;3(1):46–54. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989x.3.1.46.

  39. Cohen J. A power primer. Psychol Bull [Internet]. 1992 [cited 2019 Jun 20];112(1):155–9. Available from: http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155.

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors acknowledge, with gratitude, the librarians of the University of Pennsylvania, Maylene Kefeng Qiu and Anne M. Larrivee, for advising on the development of our search strategy.

Funding

This systematic review was developed as part of the Longitudinal Norms and Networks Study (LENNS), which is funded by the Bill Gates & Melinda Foundation (Grant No. OPP1157257). The funding agency had no role in the development of the protocol.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

JK, MGD, and ET conceived of the review concept. JK and MGD designed and developed the review protocol. JK and MGD determined the scope of the review and co-developed the search strategy, eligibility criteria, and tools, including those used to extract relevant data, assess the risk of bias, and appraise the quality of evidence. JK developed the analysis plan and was overseen by MGD. CB secured funding for and supervises LENNS. JK wrote the first draft of the protocol. All authors contributed substantially in providing comments on the protocol and have reviewed and approved the final draft.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Jinyi Kuang or Cristina Bicchieri.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Not applicable

Consent for publication

The authors provide consent for publication.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1:.

PRISMA-P checklist.

Additional file 2:.

Search strategy details.

Additional file 3:.

Data to be extracted.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Kuang, J., Delea, M.G., Thulin, E. et al. Do descriptive norms messaging interventions backfire? Protocol for a systematic review of the boomerang effect. Syst Rev 9, 267 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13643-020-01533-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13643-020-01533-0

Keywords